Swooping toward a victim with its vicious talons extended, a bird of prey homes in for the kill with meticulous precision. ‘Birds, particularly birds of prey [raptors], are believed to forage primarily using visual cues’, says vision scientist Simon Potier from Lund University, Sweden, who was inspired by his father's love of birds of prey to take up falconry as a small child. Yet, Potier explains that it was not clear how the birds use the region where the views from the two eyes overlap (the binocular zone) during their rapier-like attacks. As the hunting strategies of birds of prey span low-speed scavenging to high-speed aerial and ground prey capture, Potier, Francesco Bonadonna and Olivier Duriez from Université de Montpellier, France, and an international team of collaborators wondered whether the binocular vision of these precision hunters might vary depending on their lifestyle.Potier turned to his own collection of almost 60 birds at Les Ailes de l'Urga theme park in France and other birds from Le Grand Parc du Puy du Fou to measure the binocular visual fields of birds ranging from carrion-feeding vultures to predatory Cooper's hawks and eagles that target victims on the ground. ‘We used a non-invasive procedure to measure visual field characteristics in alert birds’, says Potier, recalling the difficulty of holding the large eagles steady for 20 min while measuring the limit of the view from each eye in order to calculate the binocular region where the visual fields overlapped. ‘The experience of the falconers who helped was essential’, he says. In addition, he had to custom build individual bill holders for each species to keep their heads in place, ‘which took me quite some time to fit perfectly’, he laughs. However, Potier was relieved when the birds appeared unfazed after they were released: ‘Directly after the experiment, they still flew to me, which indicates that they were not stressed’, he smiles.Initially, Potier and his colleagues had anticipated that the binocular visual fields of the hunters would be wider than those of the scavengers, as the hunters have to precisely target a moving victim with their talons while the scavengers’ aim may not need to be as precise. However, the width of the binocular visual fields was remarkably similar. The contrasts were more apparent when the team plotted the full projections of the binocular region, showing that the zone above the beak where the view from the two eyes overlapped tapered more rapidly in the ground-hunting eagles than it did in Cooper's hawks (which target birds on the wing) and the scavenging vultures. There was also an extensive blind zone above the heads of the ground predators – which do not have to outmanoeuvre agile targets on the wing. Considering the various hunting strategies and differences in the head shapes of the 15 species, Potier and his colleagues suspect that the ridge of bone that shades the eyes of the ground-hunting species could partially obscure the upper portion of their binocular field to produce the large blind zone. In addition, their short, but wide, powerful beaks appear to shade the lower portion of their view.‘Our results show that the binocular field configuration is specialized to foraging tactics in raptors’, says Potier, who is keen to scrutinise the vision of the almost exclusively terrestrial secretary bird. ‘They have very large eyes… I would love to estimate their visual field’, he muses.
Read full abstract