Abstract
The ability to detect and resolve conflict is an essential function of cognitive control. Laboratory studies often use stimulus–response-compatibility (SRC) tasks to examine conflict processing in order to elucidate the mechanism and modular organization of cognitive control. Inspired by two influential theories regarding cognitive control, the conflict monitoring theory (Botvinick, Braver, Barch, Carter, & Cohen, 2001) and dimensional overlap taxonomy (Kornblum, Hasbroucq, & Osman, 1990), we explored the temporal and spectral similarities and differences between processing of stimulus–stimulus (S–S) and stimulus–response (S–R) conflicts with event related potential (ERP) and time-frequency measures. We predicted that processing of S–S conflict starts earlier than that of S–R conflict and that the two types of conflict may involve different frequency bands. Participants were asked to perform two parallel SRC tasks, both combining the Stroop task (involving S–S conflict) and Simon task (involving S–R conflict). ERP results showed pronounced SRC effects (incongruent vs. congruent) on N2 and P3 components for both S–S and S–R conflicts. In both tasks, SRC effects of S–S conflict took place earlier than those of S–R conflict. Time-frequency analysis revealed that both types of SRC effects modulated theta and alpha bands, while S–R conflict effects additionally modulated power in the beta band. These results indicated that although S–S and S–R conflict processing shared considerable ERP and time-frequency properties, they differed in temporal and spectral dynamics. We suggest that the modular organization of cognitive control should take both commonality and distinction of S–S and S–R conflict processing into consideration.
Talk to us
Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have
Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.