No THOROUGH objective investigation of the psychological adjustment of obese children, as compared to non-obese children, had been done. What opinions and explorations had been made pointed to the apparent relative psychological maladjustment of the obese. Therefore, it was hypothesized that: (1) obese children as a group show significantly poorer adjustment than non-obese children; (2) obese girls show significantly poorer adjustment than obese boys; and (3) there are significant differences in adjustment among obese children in grades 3, 5,8, and Il. Psychological adjustment was divided into two areas-social adjustment (operationally defined as the score on the Ohio Social Acceptance Scale) and emotional adjustment (operationally defined as scores on the California Test qf Personality and on The Way I feel About Myself Scale). Subjects were 120 obese and 120 non-obese students, with equal representation for each sex and for grades 3, 5, 8, and 11. The cut-off point for defining obesity was the seventy-fifth percentile of weight for age and sex on the revised Stuart and Meredith Tables [l, 21 and one standard deviation above average for age and sex in skinfold thickness. Analyses of variance and, where indicated, the Newman-Keuls test and the test for treatment means were used to analyze data. With regard to social adjustment, there were no significant differences for any of the hypotheses. Only the self-concept aspect of emotional adjustment differed between the obese and non-obese, with the obese showing a poorer self-concept. Grade and sex differences were not as hypothesized. Possible explanations for the results and some implications were discussed. Initial attitudes towards the obese have been notoriously negative and suggestive of unhappiness on the part of this group. The name “fatty” is certainly almost as popular in elementary school as the name “retard”. Obviously, the obese child’s social experiences are often negative. It would seem likely that these experiences would be reflected in his or her social and emotional adjustment. In terms of social learning theory [3], these ‘distorted’ reactions serve as a set of cues associated with expectancies for certain behavioral reactions. Once a person comes to expect these reactions, he acts in a manner which is more likely to produce them or defends against them. Thus, the obese may come to expect rejection and act in a defensive manner which leads to further rejection and corresponding maladjustment. For example, groups of ten to twelve year old children (N > 600) consistently ranked children’s pictures according to their liking of them in the following order (from most to least liked): a normal child, a child with crutches and a leg brace, a child in a wheel chair, a child with a left forearm amputation, a child with a slight facial disfigurement, and a obese child [4]. Although this ordering is found among mean ranks consistently, few children actually choose the drawings in exactly this order. However, almost all chose the normal child first and the obese child last [5]. The same type of ordering was also found in a high school sample [6]. The very definite possibility that such dislike of the obese relates to their emotional well-being is pointed out by Mayer [7], who conducted a study using mainly projective techniques to investigate the feelings of 100 obese female adolescents, as compared to 65 controls. The overwhelming conclusion of the study was that there are both “striking similarities between the characteristic psychological traits of these obese adolescent girls and those of ethnic and
Read full abstract