One of the most important functions of the brain is to guide our behavior through an ever-changing environment as our goals and desires fluctuate. One simple, yet elegant system for investigating how sensory information and goal-related information combine to control and coordinate efficient behavior is the guidance of saccadic eye movements. The brain relies heavily on visual input to guide and coordinate our complex actions, but it is impossible for our brain to simultaneously process every aspect of visual information present in real world scenes at any given moment. Instead, the retina has evolved a highly specialized fovea in the center where we have the highest visual acuity. Therefore, to analyze a visual scene optimally, the eyes reorient in complex sequences so that the high acuity fovea of the retina can be directed to specific objects of interest. These saccadic eye movements are interspersed with periods of active fixation during which the visual system performs a detailed analysis of an object that may pertain to our current goals. Alternating between the serial process of making saccades and the analytical process of active fixation is repeated several hundred thousand times per day and is critical for numerous behaviors like reading this issue of EJN, driving an automobile or negotiating a busy sidewalk. In the laboratory, saccadic behavior can be measured easily and accurately. Quantitative analysis of saccade behavior can also serve as an important tool to investigate brain disorders in a host of neurological and psychiatric disorders (Leigh & Zee, 2006). Over the past few decades, there has been a tremendous increase in our knowledge of the systems that contribute to the control of saccadic eye movements. There have been many recent and exciting advances. Anatomical, physiological, clinical and imaging studies have contributed to our extensive knowledge of the saccade control circuit (Fig. 1), which includes regions of the occipital, parietal and frontal cortex, basal ganglia, thalamus, superior colliculus, cerebellum, and brainstem reticular formation. Within each of these brain regions are multiple populations of neurons and subnuclei that perform critical operations to coordinate behavior. In addition to overt eye movements that explicitly orient the visual system, visual selection may also be directed covertly to different locations or objects without any movement of the eyes. For example, we have the ability to shift attention away from where we look without initiating a saccade. Both overt (move the eyes) and covert (only shift attention) orienting can be directed voluntarily to a specific location or object, or involuntarily ‘captured’ by an abrupt change in the visual environment (Posner, 1980; Theeuwes, 1991; Fecteau & Munoz, 2006). There is significant overlap in the brain areas that participate in overt and covert orienting, which include several components of the frontoparietal network and the superior colliculus. This special issue of EJN contains reviews and original articles by some of the leading experts in the fields of saccades and visual search. There have been many recent exciting advances in these fields that are captured in the contributions in this issue. The contributions focus on different brain areas but the content ranges from the sensory input to the motor output and everything in between (Fig. 1). There are several different state-of-the-art technologies employed including quantitative behavioral analysis, neurophysiology, neuroimaging, clinical investigation, transcranial magnetic stimulation and modeling.
Read full abstract