The subsidization of predators occurs when humans directly or indirectly alter resource availability in such a way as to increase the density of a predator population above levels that would occur without the additional resources. Subsidized predators can drastically impact prey populations because subsidies insulate the predator populations from the effects of declines in prey populations (Sinclair et al., 1998). Such scenarios become problematic for conservation practitioners when the enhanced predator population influences species of conservation concern. Examples of this are widespread: subsidized ravens influence tortoise ecology (Boarman, 2003); subsidized predatory beetles influence insect herbivore populations (Rand & Louda, 2006); subsidized mountain lions influence bighorn sheep demographics (Rominger et al., 2004); and in the example presented by Shapira, Sultan & Shanas (2008), subsidized red foxes Vulpes vulpes influence gerbil ecology. Shapira et al.’s fox-gerbil example is particularly interesting because the influence of foxes is best seen not in altered densities of gerbils per se, but in the behavior of these taxa. In areas near farms, the availability of crops and favorable habitat apparently subsidize foxes. As a result, in areas where foxes are putatively more abundant, gerbils are not only less abundant but are also less active, and show decreased foraging efficiency. This suggests that foxes in this agricultural region have altered gerbil biology not by direct predation alone, but also by creating a landscape of fear (Brown, Laundre & Gurung, 1999; Laundre, Hernandez & Altendorf, 2001), from which the gerbils have somehow rendered a microhabitat-level map of the perceived risk of predation. It is harder, however, to address the effect of actual predation on gerbils as Shapira et al. do not present information on the diet of foxes. Indeed, the differential activity rates and behaviors of gerbils at sites near and far from foxes are potentially confounded by several concerns. Foxes are not uniformly distributed but are found near farms. While this is, of course, a primary point of Shapira et al.’s paper, it nonetheless means that many of the factors making the farmed areas so different from the more natural surrounding areas are also presumably correlated with fox distributions and may also influence gerbil biology. In addition, which of the two gerbil species, or the ratio of the two species, that is being examined at each study site is not clear. This is important, because if the two gerbil species differ in their foraging and behavior, then observed differences in gerbil ecology and behavior that are attributed to foxes may in fact be a function of which gerbil species is visiting the seed tray. Finally, the foraging behavior of the predator itself may differ across study sites. Shapira et al. did not study the ecology of the foxes, but we know that predator behavior is not static. Just as prey alter their behavior to address perceived risks of predation, predator behavior also changes as a function of prey density and behavior (Lima, 2002; Quinn & Cresswell, 2004). Nonetheless, differential intensity of fox use of the landscape does appear to be the most parsimonious explanation for the observed patterns of gerbil ecology, as these patterns match our understanding of how subsidized predators and their prey should interact. In a world in which anthropogenic landscape change is a seemingly ubiquitous concern for conservationists, can we predict a prioriwhere and which predators are likely to become problematic in such situations, and can we make recommendations as we attempt to identify solutions? Although a full examination of these two questions is beyond the scope of this commentary, we know enough about the ecology of predators, of prey and of their intraand interguild interactions to make some generalizations that may carry predictive power and applied relevance. For instance, we know that larger carnivores may suppress populations of other guild members in the same way in which predators influence prey: through predation and by creating landscapes of fear (Linnell & Strand, 2000; Creel, Spong & Creel, 2001). At the Israeli–Jordanian study sites, the generalist and mid-sized red fox apparently outcompetes