Abstract: This study considered true and in first-semester French and Spanish classes to: (a) determine whether true and differ in anxiety, grades, and plans to continue language study; and (b) identify classroom factors foster anxiety or comfort. Students completed a questionnaire included the Foreign Language Classroom Anxiety Scale (Horwitz, Horwitz, & Cope, 1986), MaIntyre and Gardner Anxiety Subscales (1989, 1994), demographic information, grade expectations, and open-ended questions. Randomly selected students were interviewed about their experiences in the courses. Statistical analyses revealed (a) although neither group was terribly anxious, true were significantly more anxious overall and during processing and output stages than beginners; (b) true expected and received lower grades than beginners; and (c) significantly more true than planned to continue studying the language. Comments on one written open-ended question and in the interviews pointed to the key role of the instructor in reducing anxiety. Key words: classroom environment, beginners, foreign language anxiety, foreign language placement, importance of teacher Languages: French, Spanish Introduction When college advisors suggest to students they enroll in first-semester French or Spanish, they often hear: But I haven't had French [Spanish] yet. Doesn't 101 have students who took it in high school? I'm not sure I'd be comfortable with that. This response may well be justified. Halff and Frisbie (1977) reported in a study of firstsemester language classes at the University of Illinois in the 1960s, 74% of the students had at least 2 years and 30% had 3 years of high school study. In these classes, the true received lower test scores early in the semester and had higher attrition rates than the beginners. This problematic articulation continues to exist. Lange, Prior, and Sims (1992) noted 42% of college students in beginning foreign language classes were starting over despite their 2, 3, or even 4 years of high school study. Other studies report even higher numbers of students returning to beginning language study: 44% in Spanish and 50% in French at Arizona State University (Guntermann, Hendrickson, & de Urioste, 1996); 82% in French at Emory University (Herron, Morris, secules, & Curtis, 1995). Given these figures, it is not surprising Klee and Rogers's (1989) survey of college Spanish programs found the most critical problem to be false beginners (p. 766), a finding echoed by Oukada (2001). Responses to an inquiry on the FLASC listserv about beginner enrollments in first-semester courses (July-Aug 2003) indicated most coordinators recognized the problem at their institutions. Two Spanish coordinators even estimated in the 85% to 90% range. Klee (2002) suggested college students mistakenly assume that high school instruction does not count and they begin language instruction when they arrive at the university (p. 248). This situation with large numbers of taking beginning foreign language classes has the potential to become more acute. As more and more foreign languages are offered in high schools and more colleges require them for entrance, the potential for bringing to college classes grows. In Spanish the situation is aggravated by its rising popularity. Heritage learners, who might be considered a special case of beginners, provide a new dimension in Spanish and in other languages as well. This study investigates the effects of the true beginner/false beginner dynamic in French and Spanish courses at the University of Wisconsin-Madison, which requires 2 years of high school study for entrance, has a language requirement in the largest college [Letters and Science] and, for logistical reasons, has no control beyond advising efforts based on placement tests to regulate who enters first-semester language courses. …