The abilities of animals in movement and coor dination frequently surpass our own abilities and fascinate us. New record speeds found among ani mals, when recorded by the scientific literature, tend to be widely publicized. Such is the case of the trap-jaw ant, Odontomachus bauri Emery, whose mandibles close in 0.13 s at a rate of 35.5 meters per second (ms'1) during a predatory strike, the fastest movement recorded among all animals (Patek et al. 2006). The fastest tactile reflex time belongs to the star-nosed mole, Condylura cristata L., which uses 22 nasal appendages to navigate through soil and can react in just 120 ms, when one of these appendages comes in contact with food (Catania & Remple 2004). Here, I would like to report the startle reflex time in skipper butter flies, which is among the fastest recorded for the animal kingdom and which is at least twice as fast as startle eye-blink reflex of humans. Startle reflexes have evolved as fast defensive (usually escape) responses in animals, engaging subcortical reflex mechanism that bypasses cere bral processing and voluntary movement. Be cause circuits involved are shorter, the latencies of the startle reactions are much shorter than those of voluntary reactions. In terms of elec tromyographic responses recorded in vitro, acous tic startle reflexes tend to be very short. In rats, for example, electromyographic responses can be as fast as 5-10 ms, and in humans as fast as 14 ms (Yeomans & Frankland 1995). The actual behavioral responses recorded in vivo tend to be longer. For example, in the African butterfly fish, Pantodon buchholzi Peters, the fastest response time to an acoustic startle stim ulus recorded on an electromyogram is 5 ms. The fish's fins initiate a motion 10 ms later, hence with the possible reflex time (ballistic jump) ca. 15 ms (Starosciak 2008). The eye-blink reflex of humans elicited by an air puff is 30-50 ms (e.g., Halmagyi & Gresty 1983). Compared to that, voluntary reaction time to a visual 'go' signal in humans is around 150 ms (Brown et al. 1991), and it takes an average of 700 ms for a human driver to move the foot from the accelerator to the brake peddle after seeing a red light (Green 2000). Just like butterfly fish, skipper butterflies rely on ballistic movements to evade preda tors, but they propel themselves through the air, not the water. They are, in fact, named for their quick, darting flight habits. They differ in several morphological characters from other butterflies (e.g., Ackery et al. 1999), most notably in having stockier bodies with stronger wing muscles. While photographing butterflies with a digital camera for several years, I have become convinced that skippers possess an extraordinary reflex time when startled by a photographic flash. Their star tle reflex is faster than I have observed in any other butterfly. Even though other butterflies and other insects are frequently startled by a photographic flash and flee after their picture is taken, their motor re sponse is normally slower than the shutter speed of the camera. Thus, even if they are startled by the flash, they appear still in photographs. Skippers, however, react so quickly to the flash, that their re action is frequently captured by the camera. In the specific camera settings I used in the photographs in Fig. 1, the shutter was open for 1/ 60 sec =17 ms. The flash, synchronized with the shutter, occurred during this time. During this ex posure time, skippers startled by the flash fre quently jumped into the air, and their image was captured in flight near the substrate. They tended to return to their original position imme diately, so that they appeared still to the human eye, and their movement was captured only by the camera. In pairs of photographs (Fig. 1: A1-A2, B1-B2, and C1-C2), one individual was recorded in the act of jumping above the same plant when star tled repeatedly. These shots were captured by fir ing the camera with intervals of ca. 20 s. It was noted by previous studies that habituation (de cline and disappearance of response during repet itive stimuli) occur with other types of startle re flexes (Yeomans & Frankland 1995). I observed the same with the jumping skippers: if they did not escape after the first shot, their jumping re sponse ceased after 2-3 repeated firings of the flash (Fig. 1: C3, D2). These photographs illustrate the unique quickness of skipper reflexes among all the other insects that I have photographed. They document what might be one of the fastest reflex times (<17 ms) so far recorded for the animal kingdom.
Read full abstract