Abstract

When we reach to pick up an object, our actions are effortlessly informed by the object’s spatial information, the position of our limbs, stored knowledge of the object’s material properties, and what we want to do with the object. A substantial body of evidence suggests that grasps are under the control of “automatic, unconscious” sensorimotor modules housed in the “dorsal stream” of the posterior parietal cortex. Visual online feedback has a strong effect on the hand’s in-flight grasp aperture. Previous work of ours exploited this effect to show that grasps are refractory to cued expectations for visual feedback. Nonetheless, when we reach out to pretend to grasp an object (pantomime grasp), our actions are performed with greater cognitive effort and they engage structures outside of the dorsal stream, including the ventral stream. Here we ask whether our previous finding would extend to cued expectations for haptic feedback. Our method involved a mirror apparatus that allowed participants to see a “virtual” target cylinder as a reflection in the mirror at the start of all trials. On “haptic feedback” trials, participants reached behind the mirror to grasp a size-matched cylinder, spatially coincident with the virtual one. On “no-haptic feedback” trials, participants reached behind the mirror and grasped into “thin air” because no cylinder was present. To manipulate haptic expectation, we organized the haptic conditions into blocked, alternating, and randomized schedules with and without verbal cues about the availability of haptic feedback. Replicating earlier work, we found the strongest haptic effects with the blocked schedules and the weakest effects in the randomized uncued schedule. Crucially, the haptic effects in the cued randomized schedule was intermediate. An analysis of the influence of the upcoming and immediately preceding haptic feedback condition in the cued and uncued random schedules showed that cuing the upcoming haptic condition shifted the haptic influence on grip aperture from the immediately preceding trial to the upcoming trial. These findings indicate that, unlike cues to the availability of visual feedback, participants take advantage of cues to the availability of haptic feedback, flexibly engaging pantomime, and natural modes of grasping to optimize the movement.

Highlights

  • Goal-directed grasping is multisensory and integrative in nature

  • This study examined the influences of both trial-to-trial calibration and of expectations about the availability of haptic feedback on reaching to grasp virtual objects

  • We expected iterative grasps performed without haptic feedback to take on the stereotypical characteristics of pantomimed grasps, including slower reach velocities, longer movement planning times, smaller peak grip aperture (PGA), and an exaggerated scaling of in-flight grip aperture to target size

Read more

Summary

INTRODUCTION

Goal-directed grasping is multisensory and integrative in nature. The muscle extensions and contractions that are engaged when we reach out to pick a goal object are specified by motor cortex and rely on computations performed on real-time visual and somatosensory information, stored information about the object’s function, and the agent’s intention (Creem and Proffitt, 2001; Rosenbaum et al, 2001; Frey, 2008; Camponogara and Volcic, 2019; Isa, 2019; Parikh et al, 2020). For “automatic” grasps, the selection and specification of parameters like wrist orientation, reach velocity and grasp aperture size (or the paths the fingers take) occurs with minimal awareness and minimal cognitive control, as is clearly the case for DF and in cases of action blindsight, in which the person cannot reliably perceive the geometry and spatial disposition of the goal object Cognitive supervision under these more natural circumstances maintains focus on the overarching goal of the movement (which is typically to do something with the object) rather than on the details of the unfolding movement in real-time. In the absence of cues, we should observe a greater influence of the immediately preceding haptic condition on grip aperture and no influence of the pending and unknown (to the participant) haptic condition on grip aperture

Participants
Procedure
Schedule
DISCUSSION
ETHICS STATEMENT

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.