Abstract

Although the control of meaningful gestures is one of the most left-lateralized functions, the relative contribution of the two hemispheres to their processing is still debated. We tested the effects of primes appearing in the left or right visual field in the form of pictures (Experiment 1), and words (Experiment 2) on categorization of movies showing intransitive (“communicative”) gestures, tool use (transitive) pantomimes, and meaningless movements. Fifteen participants (eight women) watched 36 movies (12 from each category) primed for 150 ms with either a congruent or incongruent stimulus followed by a 50-ms mask. On congruent trials, a picture or word was directly related to the presented gesture, including nonsense pictures or non-words for meaningless actions. On incongruent trials, a picture or word belonged to a different category. In Experiment 1, intransitive gestures were categorized significantly faster than the other two types of hand movements. Moreover, whereas the categorization of transitive gestures was significantly facilitated by congruent pictures on the right, the effect was weaker for intransitive, and reversed for meaningless movements. In Experiment 2, intransitive gestures were again categorized significantly faster, but transitive significantly slower than the other two gesture categories. Yet, there was now a significant facilitation of intransitive, and inhibition of transitive gesture categorization following congruent prime words in the right visual field, and significantly faster categorization of intransitive gestures following incongruent words in the left visual field. These outcomes lend support to the complexity account of differences in left-hemisphere representations of meaningful gestures reported in the neuropsychological, behavioral, and neuroimaging literature. Nevertheless, they also indicate that the representations of intransitive gestures show some differential, and sometimes counterintuitive sensitivity to right hemisphere processing.

Highlights

  • Our current knowledge on the laterality of representations underlying meaningful gestures comes primarily from research on patients with acquired brain injuries, and more recently from experiments using functional neuroimaging

  • Except for the main effect of gesture [F(2, 28) = 8.1, p < 0.01; Partial Eta Squared = 0.37; observed power = 0.94] such that both intransitive and transitive gestures were categorized with significantly greater accuracy than meaningless hand movements (Bf-p < 0.01, and Bf-p < 0.05, respectively), all the remaining significant main effects and interactions were driven by differences in the categorization of meaningless actions. (These are of no particular interest in the absence of significant differences between the two meaningful gesture categories.) The average categorization accuracy for intransitive gestures was 83% (SE = 2%), for transitive gestures it was 85% (SE = 2.8%), and for meaningless hand movements it was only 73% (SE = 1.7%)

  • To Experiment 1, there was a main effect of gesture [F(2, 28) = 4.8, p < 0.05; pη2 = 0.25; alpha = 0.75] but it was such that only intransitive gestures were categorized with significantly greater accuracy than meaningless hand movements (Bf-p < 0.05), whereas the difference between transitive and meaningless actions did not reach significance level (Bf-p = 0.2)

Read more

Summary

Introduction

Our current knowledge on the laterality of representations underlying meaningful gestures comes primarily from research on patients with acquired brain injuries, and more recently from experiments using functional neuroimaging (for reviews, see Frey, 2008; Rumiati et al, 2010; Goldenberg, 2013a; see Goldenberg, 2013b; Króliczak, 2013a). These studies overwhelmingly point to the left hemisphere as the seat of the control of gesture.

Methods
Results
Discussion
Conclusion
Full Text
Paper version not known

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.