Abstract
The “hub and spoke model” of semantic representation suggests that the multimodal features of objects are drawn together by an anterior temporal lobe (ATL) “hub”, while modality-specific “spokes” capture perceptual/action features. However, relatively little is known about how these components are recruited through time to support object identification. We used magnetoencephalography to measure neural oscillations within left ATL, lateral fusiform cortex (FC) and central sulcus (CS) during word-picture matching at different levels of specificity (employing superordinate vs. specific labels) for different categories (manmade vs. animal). This allowed us to determine (i) when each site was sensitive to semantic category and (ii) whether this was modulated by task demands. In ATL, there were two phases of response: from around 100 ms post-stimulus there were phasic bursts of low gamma activity resulting in reductions in oscillatory power, relative to a baseline period, that were modulated by both category and specificity; this was followed by more sustained power decreases across frequency bands from 250 ms onwards. In the spokes, initial power increases were not stronger for specific identification, while later power decreases were stronger for specific-level identification in FC for animals and in CS for manmade objects (from around 150 ms and 200 ms, respectively). These data are inconsistent with a temporal sequence in which early sensory-motor activity is followed by later retrieval in ATL. Instead, knowledge emerges from the rapid recruitment of both hub and spokes, with early specificity and category effects in the ATL hub. The balance between these components depends on semantic category and task, with visual cortex playing a greater role in the fine-grained identification of animals and motor cortex contributing to the identification of tools.
Highlights
Conceptual processing plays a crucial role in our lives, allowing us to understand the significance of words and objects and to guide our behaviour [1, 2]
The results from the behavioural pilot are reported in S4 Fig. Participants were slower when making a superordinate as opposed to a specific level judgement, and when categorising manmade objects
There was an interaction between these factors–the slowest responses occurred in the manmade superordinate condition, perhaps reflecting the featural diversity of manmade objects relative to animals
Summary
Conceptual processing plays a crucial role in our lives, allowing us to understand the significance of words and objects and to guide our behaviour [1, 2]. The view that knowledge is captured in the links between different motor and sensory representations is supported by a wealth of neuroimaging studies that have shown differential patterns of activation for concepts that draw on different types of features: thinking of a rose produces activation in cortical regions linked to colour and smell processing (alongside other regions), while thinking of a tennis racquet elicits additional areas of activity in regions linked to action and praxis [6, 9,10,11] This principle may underpin category-specific effects in conceptual processing, since visual and motor/praxis features are likely to be important for differentiating animals and manipulable manmade objects respectively [12,13,14]. Manmade objects have more diverse visual features at the superordinate level, and might not show the same interaction between visual processing and specificity [17,18,19,20], instead, when artefacts must be identified as a ‘nut-cracker’ or a ‘knife’, the different actions and grips associated with these objects may be crucial for distinguishing them [17, 21,22,23]
Talk to us
Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have
Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.