Abstract

General anaesthetics reversibly produce unconsciousness and block motor response to noxious stimuli. These drugs are among the most useful in modern medicine. They not only prevent pain during surgery but also permit operations of a complexity that was unimaginable 150 years ago. What is perhaps less appreciated is that these drugs play a similar role for in vivo experimentation by providing pain-free animal and ideal testing conditions for the experimenter. However, anaesthetics pose a particular problem for the neuroscientist because they influence the activity of the system under study. The nature and magnitude of this influence are rarely known with confidence. These factors depend on the structure being studied, the species of the animal as well as on the type and dose of anaesthetic agents. The main target of general anaesthetic action is the synapse. The most common direct effect of general anaesthetics is to enhance inhibitory and attenuate excitatory transmission (Rudolph & Antkowiak, 2004). Furthermore, these drugs also exert a myriad of secondary and higher order actions because of the immense level of interconnectivity in the CNS (Eckenhoff & Johansson, 1999). It is thus surprising (and fortunate) that our knowledge of brain function and sensory processing, the vast majority of which was accumulated from experiments on anaesthetized animals, applies, for the most part, to the awake, behaving condition (for example see Snodderly & Gur, 1995, for visual processing). There are, however, exceptions, and some higher order processes are affected by general anaesthesia (Lamme et al. 1998; Pack et al. 2001). It is thus puzzling that many neuroscientists working with anaesthetized animals seem to have only a modest interest for studies primarily aimed at the neurophysiological effects of general anaesthetics, even though these may have implications for their own work. Neuroscientists have so little interest for general anaesthetics that the name of the anaesthetic used is on occasions relegated to the on-line material and not even mentioned in the article (Logothetis et al. 2001; Bruno & Sakmann, 2006). This lack of interest for anaesthesia is also reflected by the very frequent omission to measure the concentration of inhaled anaesthetic, even when the process under study is known to be influenced by general anaesthetics (for example, see the very interesting work on gamma oscillations by Barth & MacDonald (1996)) Musizza et al. (2007) are to be commended for making the effects of general anaesthetics the central theme of their article in this issue of The Journal of Physiology. Using complex tools from non-linear analysis and information theory, they studied interactions between neural (EEG), respiratory and cardiac oscillations in rats anaesthetized with ketamine and xylazine or pentobarbital, agents that are commonly used in neuroscience investigations. They showed that, for both groups, respiration drives the cardiac oscillator during deep anaesthesia and that the transition from deep to light anaesthesia is accompanied by an increase in θ wave activity. With ketamine–xylazine, the cardio-respiratory interaction either reverses or becomes negligible; with pentobarbital the interactions become weaker during light anaesthesia without other observable changes. These findings thus provide a detailed characterization of the anaesthetic state. The authors also deserve praise for having included the cardiac and respiratory oscillations to obtain a wider view than that provided by exclusive attention to the EEG. Although the anaesthetic influences on cardiac and respiratory control have been recognized for a long time (Guedel, 1937; Biscoe & Millar, 1966) and reflect of the ubiquity of anaesthetic action, most studies attempting to understand how general anaesthetics impair brain function limit their scope to one modality. Here are suggestions for future studies. For animal experiments in line with the very interesting work of Musizza et al. (2007), I would suggest the study of chronically instrumented animals to obtain awake baseline and recovery data. Consideration should also be given to precise control of the concentration of the anaesthetic agent. The control of concentration makes it possible to adjust the level of anaesthesia, to keep it constant, to control its duration and to include different levels within each experimental session. The control of concentration is easily done for inhaled drugs with the combination of airtight recording chambers and commercially available anaesthetic gas analysers that provide on-line measure of anaesthetic concentration. For parenteral drugs, such as those used by Musizza et al. (2007) the concentration can be adjusted by the combination of intravenous administration via chronic intravenous catheters and computer-controlled infusions based on pharma-cokinetic data (Shafer & Gregg, 1992). For investigations with patients, I would suggest including measurements of heart rate and respiration because these are readily provided by the clinical monitors. Wilder Penfield once wrote ‘The problem of neurology is to understand man himself.’ I will conclude by paraphrasing him: ‘Investigations of the neurophysiological effects of general anaesthetics aim to understand the nervous system itself.’

Full Text
Paper version not known

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.