Abstract

Research shows that people’s wait-and-see preferences for actions against climate change are a result of several factors, including cognitive misconceptions. The use of simulation tools could help reduce these misconceptions concerning Earth’s climate. However, it is still unclear whether the learning in these tools is of the problem’s surface features (dimensions of emissions and absorptions and cover-story used) or of the problem’s structural features (how emissions and absorptions cause a change in CO2 concentration under different CO2 concentration scenarios). Also, little is known on how problem’s difficulty in these tools (the shape of CO2 concentration trajectory), as well as the use of these tools as a decision aid influences performance. The primary objective of this paper was to investigate how learning about Earth’s climate via simulation tools is influenced by problem’s surface and structural features, problem’s difficulty, and decision aids. In experiment 1, we tested the influence of problem’s surface and structural features in a simulation called Dynamic Climate Change Simulator (DCCS) on subsequent performance in a paper-and-pencil Climate Stabilization (CS) task (N = 100 across four between-subject conditions). In experiment 2, we tested the effects of problem’s difficulty in DCCS on subsequent performance in the CS task (N = 90 across three between-subject conditions). In experiment 3, we tested the influence of DCCS as a decision aid on subsequent performance in the CS task (N = 60 across two between-subject conditions). Results revealed a significant reduction in people’s misconceptions in the CS task after performing in DCCS compared to when performing in CS task in the absence of DCCS. The decrease in misconceptions in the CS task was similar for both problems’ surface and structural features, showing both structure and surface learning in DCCS. However, the proportion of misconceptions was similar across both simple and difficult problems, indicating the role of cognitive load to hamper learning. Finally, misconceptions were reduced when DCCS was used as a decision aid. Overall, these results highlight the role of simulation tools in alleviating climate misconceptions. We discuss the implication of using simulation tools for climate education and policymaking.

Highlights

  • Understanding stocks and flows is a fundamental process in the real world (Dörner, 1996; Sterman, 2008, 2011; Cronin et al, 2009; Fischer et al, 2015)

  • Recent research has shown that structural knowledge helps people reduce their correlation heuristic and violation of mass balance misconceptions in both cases when problems encountered in the Climate Stabilization (CS) task are structurally similar or different compared to those presented in Dynamic Climate Change Simulator (DCCS) (Kumar and Dutt, unpublished)

  • The dependent variables were the proportion of participants relying on correlation heuristic and the proportion of participants committing violation of mass balance

Read more

Summary

Introduction

Understanding stocks and flows is a fundamental process in the real world (Dörner, 1996; Sterman, 2008, 2011; Cronin et al, 2009; Fischer et al, 2015). Different stock-flow problems share the same underlying structure: A stock or level accumulates the inflows to it less the outflows from it (Sweeney and Sterman, 2000) It is a well-known phenomenon that people have difficulties in understanding the dynamics of stock-flow problems (Dörner, 1996; Sterman, 2008, 2011; Cronin et al, 2009; Dutt, 2011). Prior research in stock-flow problems concerning Earth’s climate has analyzed reliance on correlation heuristic and violation of mass balance in the CS task (Sterman and Sweeney, 2007; Sterman, 2008; Dutt and Gonzalez, 2012a) (see Figure 1). This paper uses the CS task with different CO2 concentration trajectories and cover stories to evaluate people’s reliance on correlation heuristic and violation of mass balance misconceptions

Objectives
Methods
Results
Conclusion
Full Text
Paper version not known

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.