Abstract

BackgroundLanguage delay is one of the major referral criteria for an autism evaluation. Once an autism spectrum diagnosis is established, the language prognosis is among the main parental concerns. Early language regression (ELR) is observed by 10–50% of parents but its relevance to late language level and socio-communicative ability is uncertain. This study aimed to establish the predictive value of ELR on the progression of language development and socio-communicative outcomes to guide clinicians in addressing parents’ concerns at the time of diagnosis.MethodsWe used socio-communicative, language, and cognitive data of 2,047 autism spectrum participants from the Simons Simplex Collection, aged 4–18 years (mean = 9 years; SD = 3.6). Cox proportional hazard and logistic regression models were used to evaluate the effect of ELR on language milestones and the probability of using complex and flexible language, as defined by the choice of ADOS module at enrollment. Linear models were then used to evaluate the relationship of ELR and non-verbal IQ with socio-communicative and language levels.ResultsELR is associated with earlier language milestones but delayed attainment of fluent, complex, and flexible language. However, this language outcome can be expected for almost all autistic children without intellectual disability at 18 years of age. It is mostly influenced by non-verbal IQ, not ELR. The language and socio-communicative level of participants with flexible language, as measured by the Vineland and ADOS socio-communicative subscales, was not affected by ELR.LimitationsThis study is based on a relatively coarse measure of ultimate language level and relies on retrospective reporting of early language milestones and ELR. It does not prospectively document the age at which language catches up, the relationship between ELR and other behavioral areas of regression, nor the effects of intervention.ConclusionsFor autistic individuals with ELR and a normal level of non-verbal intelligence, language development follows a “bayonet shape” trajectory: early first words followed by regression, a plateau with limited progress, and then language catch up.

Highlights

  • One of the first parental concerns leading to an assessment for possible autism is a delay or atypicalities in language and communication [1, 2]

  • For autistic individuals with early language regression (ELR) and a normal level of non-verbal intelligence, language development follows a “bayonet shape” trajectory: early first words followed by regression, a plateau with limited progress, and language catch up

  • intellectual quotient (IQ) characteristics of ELR vs No‐ELR autistic children Overall, participants with ELR had a lower IQ than NoELR participants (NVIQ: p = 3.3e−13; ELR after their first words (ELR-W): 78, ELRP: 79, No-ELR: 89; Verbal IQ (VIQ): p = 1.3e−21; ELR-W: 68, Early lan‐ guage regression after first phrases (ELR-P): 70, No-ELR: 85), which was true when restricted to fluent speakers (NVIQ: p = 0.038; ELR: 92; No-ELR: 96; VIQ: p = 4.6e−4; ELR: 88, No-ELR: 95)

Read more

Summary

Introduction

One of the first parental concerns leading to an assessment for possible autism is a delay or atypicalities in language and communication [1, 2]. The aim of this study was to help clinicians respond to questions about language prognosis at the time of diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder with early language regression (ELR). After apparently normal language and motor development [10], from 10 to 50% of parents of children later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder note a loss of previously acquired words [11,12,13,14,15,16,17,18]. This study aimed to establish the predictive value of ELR on the progression of language development and socio-communicative outcomes to guide clinicians in addressing parents’ concerns at the time of diagnosis

Objectives
Methods
Results
Conclusion
Full Text
Published version (Free)

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call