BACKGROUND: Assessing recovery after general anesthesia is complicated because patients must be sufficiently arousable to pay attention to the verbal instructions probing cognitive function. All delirium screens that have been used in the postanesthesia care unit (PACU) rely heavily on a patient’s ability to process language information and respond appropriately. However, postanesthesia effects on speech-language functions have not been specifically evaluated. Here we present a novel, qualitative PACU evaluation for cognitive domains critical for speech-language functions, and we compare this assessment against a multidomain neurocognitive examination: Telephonic Montreal Cognitive Assessment (t-MoCA). This may be used to identify trajectories of neurocognitive recovery after surgery with general anesthesia and provide neuroanatomic correlates for specific deficits. METHODS: We investigated 48 patients undergoing general anesthesia for noncardiac and nonneurologic elective surgeries. Preoperatively, participants were administered our PACU speech-language assessment (PACU-SLA) and t-MoCA. Both assessments were again administered postoperatively in the PACU. Different versions of PACU-SLA were administered pre- versus postoperatively. Twenty-three participants randomly received the same t-MoCA versions (group AA), and 25 participants received different versions (group AB), pre- versus postoperatively. Assessments were administered ≥30 minutes after PACU arrival, and before PACU discharge. Statistical analysis was performed using Wilcoxon-signed-rank tests for nonnormally distributed paired data, analysis of covariance for assessing the impact of group (AA versus AB) and preoperative scores on postoperative scores, and χ2 tests for unpaired categorical data (P < .05 indicating significance). RESULTS: After adjusting for preoperative scores, the postoperative t-MoCA scores of group AB were significantly lower than group AA (F[1–46] = 21.7, P < .001). Similarly, the t-MoCA scores of episodic-memory (delayed-recall) decreased in group AB (F[1–46] = 48.6, P < .001). For PACU-SLA, there were no postoperative changes in auditory-comprehension and object-naming scores, but a decrease was observed in (1) scores of a 9-point narrative-production task of expressive-fluency (n = 48; median [25th–75th]; preoperative: 9[9–9], postoperative: 7[7–8], P < .001), and (2) total words generated in 2 30-second tasks of verbal-fluency (n = 48; median[25th–75th]: preoperative: 23[12.5–33.5], postoperative: 16.5[9.5–26.5], P < .001). Scores on a 4-point sentence-repetition task were also noted to decrease postoperatively (n = 48; median[25th–75th]; preoperative: 4[3–4], postoperative: 4[3–4], P = .04). When grouping participants by preoperative cognitive status (pMCI, n = 9; preoperative normal, n = 39), both groups showed postoperative changes in verbal-fluency (F[1–46] = 6.97, P = .01) and narrative-production scores (F[1–46] = 5.51, P = .02). CONCLUSIONS: The PACU-SLA revealed lower fluency (hypophonia) with relatively intact comprehension, naming, and repetition, during recovery from general anesthesia. These deficits share features with transcortical motor aphasia.
Read full abstract