McSweeney and Weatherly (1998) suggested that multiple-schedule behavioral contrast may be explained, at least partly, by differences in the amount of habituation to the reinforcer produced during the baseline and contrast phases of a contrast experiment. The present experiment studied behavioral contrast when pigeons pecked keys for food reinforcers delivered on multiple VI VI schedules. The results confirmed two predictions made by McSweeney and Weatherly. Both positive and negative contrast were larger for longer (i.e. 80 min) than for shorter sessions (i.e. 20 min). Positive and negative contrast were also larger later, rather than earlier, in the session. These results support the conclusion that factors that weaken the effectiveness of the reinforcer with its repeated delivery (e.g., habituation) may be among the factors contributing to multiple-schedule behavioral contrast. Key words: behavioral contrast, dynamic changes, session length, habituation Behavioral contrast refers to an inverse relationship between the rate of reinforcement in one component of a multiple schedule and the rate of responding in the other component. Positive contrast occurs when decreasing the rate of reinforcement in one component is associated with increases in the rate of responding in the other component. Negative contrast occurs when increasing the rate of reinforcement in one component is associated with decreases in the rate of responding in the other component. ********** Understanding behavioral contrast is important for several reasons. Contrast is important theoretically because it shows that the effects of reinforcers are relative rather than absolute. For this reason, it plays a pivotal role in several operant theories (e.g. Herrnstein, 1970). Contrast is important practically because it can occur in applied settings (e.g., Gross & Drabman, 1981). In some cases, behavioral contrast may work against the goals of therapy. Koegal, Egel and Williams (1980) found that reinforcing a target behavior by autistic children in a therapy setting was accompanied by decreases in this behavior in another, non-therapy setting (e.g., home, a classroom). If the goal of therapy is to change the frequency of a target behavior across various contexts, such instances of contrast interfere with this aim. In other cases, contrast may facilitate the goals of therapy. Charlop, Kurtz and Milstein (1992) showed that putting maintenance tasks (i.e. tasks that their subjects had been performing at approximately 80% accuracy for 3 months prior to the study) on extinction was associated with increases in the frequency of a designated acquisition task (i.e. a task the child had never been asked to do prior to the experiment). All six autistic children reached performance criteria for the acquisition task using this procedure. Although behavioral contrast has been extensively studied, the theoretical factors that contribute(s) to this phenomenon remain the topic of debate (see Williams, 1983, 2002, for a review). Most theories of contrast argue that contrast arises from a comparison of the rates of reinforcement in the two components of the multiple schedule when those components provide different rates of reinforcement. For example, the additive theories (e.g., Gamzu & Schwartz, 1973; Hearst & Jenkins, 1974; Rachlin, 1973) argue that contrast occurs when subjects make transitions between components that provide higher or lower valued reinforcers. Response interference theories (e.g., Hinson & Staddon, 1978) argue that the difference in the rates of reinforcement between the components leads to the reallocation of responding across those components. McSweeney and Weatherly (1998) attribute behavioral contrast to a different comparison (e.g., different overall rates of reinforcement received during different phases of the experiment) Specifically, they suggest that behavioral contrast occurs, at least partly, because the procedure commonly used to study contrast (e. …
Read full abstract