Many continental populations of the Loggerhead Shrike (Lanius ludovicianus) have been declining in abundance for a number of years (Robbins et al. 1986, Brooks and Temple 1990, Lowe and Butcher 1990), and the San Clemente Island subspecies (L. 1. mearnsi) in California is highly endangered (Scott and Morrison 1990). Consequently, biologists have become increasingly interested in using manipulative techniques to study factors underlying the decline and to augment or restore wild populations. These techniques include the release of captive-raised and captive-bred shrikes, the translocation of adults, their eggs and young, and related manipulations (Kuehler et al., in press). It seems worthwhile, therefore, to report some details on the successful breeding of this species in captivity in 1971, especially as the loggerhead has not previously bred in captivity, and the few successful attempts with Eurasian species (L. collurio, L. excubitor, L. schach, and L. vittatus) have, with one exception (Giinther 1904), involved wild-caught, adult pairs (England 1970, 197 la, 1971b; Weischner 1989). The breeding pair of loggerheads came from a group of eight young shrikes taken from nests in northeastern Colorado in June, 1970, and hand-reared from the age of 8-9 days. These shrikes were transported to Dryden, New York as nestlings and were kept together until late September, when increasing aggression necessitated separation. During this period the birds were trained (Cade 1962, 1967) and allowed daily flights outside their cage as part of a study of the development of their hunting and impaling behavior. During fall and winter, individuals were housed indoors in separate, wire-mesh cages measuring 1.2 m in each dimension. The cages were arranged so that all the birds could see and hear each other. Five birds contracted avian pox and died during the winter; the three survivors were two males and a female. In mid-April, 1971 one of the male shrikes began a daily routine of singing and displaying in an extreme upright posture, leaning somewhat backward with its beak pointed straight up, and often turning slowly to one side or the other in this extreme upright posture while quivering its wings and singing. The behavior appeared identical to that of captive Northern Shrikes (Lanius excubitor) observed previously (Cade 1962). On 19 April, this bird was transferred to the Behavioral Ecology Building (Hawk Barn) at the Cornell University Laboratory of Omrnithology and placed in a chamber measuring 3 m wide by 6 m long and varying from 4.2-6 m high. The room had been provided with several freshly cut, leafless willow (Salix sp.) shrubs and hawthorns (Crataegus sp.) ranging from 2-3 m high to simulate a natural environment. Some branchlets were broken and sharpened to provide convenient impaling devices. Throughout the breeding season, laboratory mice, nestling sparrows and starlings, and dayold cockerels were provided for food. In addition, some insects (beetles, wasps, flies) entered the room from an open, wired front and were eaten. Later, an old but well-formed nest of the Brown Thrasher (Toxostoma rufum) was affixed 1.5 m up against the trunk of a hawthorn, and twigs, dry grass, cotton tufts, and wool yarn were placed on the floor for nesting materials. In a few days, the male appeared at ease in his new chamber, singing and displaying from several prominent perches. On 27 April, the female shrike was let loose in the chamber. She perched low in the middle of a hawthorn and did not move. The male flew down and hopped all around in branches near her. She did not flee, vocalize, or attempt to fight but sat still in a I Received 13 April 1992. Accepted 27 July 1992. SHORT COMMUNICATIONS 1027
Read full abstract