and stimulus similarity, Psychological Review, 96, 433-458 (1989); H. Pashler, Target-distractor dis criminability in visual search, Perception & Psycho physics, 41, 285-292 (1987). 5. M.L. Shaw, Attending to multiple sources of information, Cognitive Psychology, 14, 353-409 (1982); M.L. Shaw, Division of attention among spatial locations: A fundamental difference between detection of letters and detection of luminance in crements, in Attention & Performance X, H. Bouma and D.G. Bouwhais, Eds. (Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 1984); for contrasting results, see P.J. Bennett and P.D. Jaye, Letter localization, not discrimination, is constrained by attention, Canadian Journal of Exper imental Psychology (in press). For more general models, see N. Graham, P. Kramer, and D. Yager, Signal-detection models for multidimensional stim uli: Probability distributions and combination rules, Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 31, 366-409 (1987). 6. M.L Posner, C.R.R. Snyder, and B.J. David son, Attention and the detection of signals, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109,160-174 (1980). 7. Shaw (1984), note 5. 8. A. Treisman and G. Gelade, A feature integration theory of attention, Cognitive Psychol ogy, 12, 97-136 (1980); D. Aiken and J. Palmer, Pursuing an alternative theory of the difference be tween conjunctions and disjunctions in visual search, unpublished manuscript, University of Washington, Seattle. 9. M. Pavel, A statistical model of preattentive visual search, paper presented at the meeting of the Psychonomic Society, New Orleans (November 1990); J. Palmer, Visual search latency: The influ ence of target-distractor discriminability on the mag nitude of set-size effects, paper presented at the meeting of the Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology, Sarasota, FL (May 1994) (tran script available from the author). 10. W.S. Geisler and K.L. Chou, The separation of low-level and high-level factors in complex tasks: Visual search, Psychological Review, 102, 356-378 (1995).
Read full abstract