Abstract

Research ArticlesWhat Does the Implicit Association Test Measure? A Test of the Convergent and Discriminant Validity of Prejudice-Related IATsBertram GawronskiBertram Gawronski University of Würzburg, Germany Search for more papers by this authorPublished OnlineApril 15, 2015https://doi.org/10.1026//1618-3169.49.3.171PDFView Full Text ToolsAdd to favoritesDownload CitationsTrack Citations ShareShare onFacebookTwitterLinkedInReddit SectionsMoreAbstractAbstract. Drawing on recent criticism of the Implicit Association Test (IAT), the present study tested the convergent and discriminant validity of two prejudice-related IATs to corresponding explicit prejudice measures in a German student sample (N = 61). Confirming convergent validity, (a) an IAT designed to assess negative associations related to Turkish people was significantly related to the explicit endorsement of prejudiced beliefs about Turkish people, and (b) an IAT designed to assess negative associations related to East Asians was significantly related to explicit prejudice against East Asians. Moreover, confirming discriminant validity, (c) the Asian IAT was unrelated to the explicit endorsement of prejudiced beliefs about Turkish people, and (d) the Turkish IAT was unrelated to explicit prejudice against Asian people. These results further corroborate the assumption that the IAT is a valid method to assess the strength of evaluative associations in the domain of prejudice and stereotypes.ReferencesAllport, D. A., Styles, E. A., Hsieh, S.(1994). Shifting intentional set: Exploring the dynamic control of tasks. In C. Umilta & M. Moscovitch (Eds.), Attention and performance XV: Conscious and nonconscious information processing 421– 452 Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. First citation in articleGoogle ScholarBanaji, I. V.(2001). Implicit stereotypes and prejudice. In G. B. Moskowitz (Ed.), Cognitive social psychology 359– 374 Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle ScholarBanse, R., Gawronski, B.(2001). Motivation zur Vorurteilskontrolle: Skaleneigenschaften und Validierung [Motivation to control prejudice: Scale properties and validation]. Manuscript submitted for publication. First citation in articleGoogle ScholarBanse, R., Seise, J., Zerbes, N.(2001). Implicit attitudes towards homosexuality: Reliability, validity, and controllability of the IAT. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 145– 160 First citation in articleLink, Google ScholarBargh, J. A., Chaiken, S., Govender, R., Pratto, F.(1992). The generality of the automatic attitude activation effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 893– 912 First citation in articleCrossref Medline, Google ScholarBrendl, C. M., Markman, A. B., Messner, C.(2001). How do indirect measures of evaluation work? Evaluating the inference of prejudice in the Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 760– 773 First citation in articleCrossref Medline, Google ScholarCampbell, D. T., Fiske, D. W.(1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56, 81– 105 First citation in articleCrossref Medline, Google ScholarCoenders, M., Scheepers, P., Sniderman, P. M., Verberk, G.(2001). Blatant and subtle prejudice: Dimensions, determinants, and consequences. European Journal of Social Psychology, 31, 281– 297 First citation in articleCrossref, Google ScholarCunningham, W. A., Preacher, K. J., Banaji, M. R.(2001). Implicit attitude measures: Consistency, stability, and convergent validity. Psychological Science, 12, 163– 170 First citation in articleCrossref Medline, Google ScholarDasgupta, N., McGhee, D. E., Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R.(2000). Automatic preference for white Americans: Eliminating the familiarity explanation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36, 316– 328 First citation in articleCrossref, Google ScholarDe Houwer, J.(2001, September). Indirect measures of attitudes. Paper presented at the 3rd Meeting of the European Social Cognition Network, Houffalize, Belgium. First citation in articleGoogle ScholarDevine, P. G.(1989). Stereotypes and prejudice: Their automatic and controlled components. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 5– 18 First citation in articleCrossref, Google ScholarDovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., Beach, K. R.(2001). Implicit and explicit attitudes: Examination of the relationship between measures of intergroup bias. In R. Brown & S. L. Gaertner (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of social psychology: Intergroup processes 175– 197 Malden, MA: Blackwell. First citation in articleGoogle ScholarDunton, B. C., Fazio, R. H.(1997). An individual difference measure of motivation to control prejudiced reactions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 316– 326 First citation in articleCrossref, Google ScholarFazio, R. H.(1993). Variability in the likelihood of automatic attitude activation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 753– 758 First citation in articleCrossref Medline, Google ScholarFazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., Williams, C. J.(1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline?. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 1013– 1027 First citation in articleCrossref Medline, Google ScholarFazio, R. H., Towles-Schwen, T.(1999). The MODE model of attitude-behavior processes. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual process theories in social psychology 97– 116 New York: Guilford Press. First citation in articleGoogle ScholarFlorack, A., Scarabis, M., Bless, H.(2001a). When do associations matter? The use of automatic associations toward ethnic groups in person judgments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 518– 524 First citation in articleCrossref, Google ScholarFlorack, A., Scarabis, M., Bless, H.(2001b). Der Einfluss wahrgenommener Bedrohung auf die Nutzung automatischer Assoziationen bei der Personenbeurteilung [The impact of perceived threat on the use of automatic associations in person judgments]. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 32, 249– 259 First citation in articleLink, Google ScholarFranco, F. M., Maass, A.(1999). Intentional control over prejudice: When the choice of measure matters. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29, 469– 477 First citation in articleCrossref, Google ScholarGawronski, B., Ehrenberg, K., Banse, R., Zukova, J., Klauer, K. C. (in press) It’s in the mind of the beholder: The impact of stereotypic associations on category-based and individuating impression formation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, First citation in articleGoogle ScholarGawronski, B., Geschke, D., Banse, R.(2002b). Implicit bias in impression formation: Associations influence the construal of individuating information. Manuscript submitted for publication. First citation in articleGoogle ScholarGawronski, B., Petzold, I., Banse, R.(2002a). Parallel context effects on implicit and explicit attitudes: Evidence for mutually independent processes. Manuscript submitted for publication. First citation in articleGoogle ScholarGreenwald, A. G., Banaji, M.(1995). Implicit social cognition: Attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychological Review, 102, 4– 27 First citation in articleCrossref Medline, Google ScholarGreenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., Schwartz, J. K. L(1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464– 1480 First citation in articleCrossref Medline, Google ScholarJarvis, W. B. G(2001). DirectRT v2002. New York: Empirisoft. First citation in articleGoogle ScholarJörsekog, K. G., Sörbom, D.(2001). LISREL 8.51. Chicago, IL: Scientific Software International, Inc. First citation in articleGoogle ScholarKlauer, K. C., Musch, J.(1999). Eine Normierung unterschiedlicher Aspekte der evaluativen Bewertung von 92 Substantiven [A standardization of various aspects of the evaluation of 92 nouns]. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 30, 1– 11 First citation in articleLink, Google ScholarMarsh, K. L., Johnson, B. T., Scott-Sheldon, L. A. J(2001). Heart versus reason in condom use: Implicit versus explicit predictors of sexual behavior. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 161– 175 First citation in articleLink, Google ScholarMcConnell, A. R., Leibold, J. M.(2001). Relations among the Implicit Association Test, discriminatory behavior, and explicit measures of racial attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 37, 435– 442 First citation in articleCrossref, Google ScholarMierke, J., Klauer, K. C.(2001). Implicit association measurement with the IAT: Evidence for effects of executive control processes. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 107– 122 First citation in articleLink, Google ScholarNeumann, R., Seibt, B.(2001). The structure of prejudice: Associative strength as a determinant of stereotype endorsement. European Journal of Social Psychology, 31, 609– 620 First citation in articleCrossref, Google ScholarNosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R.(2001). The go/no-go association task. Social Cognition, 19, 625– 664 First citation in articleCrossref, Google ScholarOttaway, S. A., Hayden, D. C., Oakes, M. A.(2001). Implicit attitudes and racism: Effects of word familiarity and frequency of the Implicit Association Test. Social Cognition, 19, 97– 144 First citation in articleCrossref, Google ScholarPettigrew, T. F., Meertens, R. W.(1995). Subtle and blatant prejudice in western Europe. European Journal of Social Psychology, 25, 57– 75 First citation in articleCrossref, Google ScholarRogers, R. D., Monsell, S.(1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 207– 231 First citation in articleCrossref, Google ScholarRothermund, K., Wentura, D.(2001). Figure ground asymmetries in the Implicit Association Test. Zeitschrift für Experimentelle Psychologie, 48, 94– 106 First citation in articleLink, Google ScholarSchwibbe, M., Räder, K., Schwibbe, G., Borchardt, M., Geiken-Pophanken, G.(1994). Zum emotionalen Gehalt von Substantiven, Adjektiven und Verben [The emotional meaning of nouns, adjectives, and verbs]. In W. Hager, & M. Hasselhorn (Eds.), Handbuch deutschsprachiger Wortnormen 272– 284 Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe. First citation in articleGoogle ScholarSmith, E. R., Zarate, M. A.(1992). Exemplar-based model of social judgment. Psychological Review, 99, 3– 21 First citation in articleCrossref, Google ScholarSmith, R. L., Ager, J. W., Williams, D. L.(1992). Suppressor variables in multiple regression/correlation. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 52, 17– 28 First citation in articleCrossref, Google ScholarSteiger, J. H.(1980). Tests for comparing elements of a correlation matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 87, 245– 251 First citation in articleCrossref, Google ScholarStrack, F.(1992). The different routes to social judgments: Experiential versus informational strategies. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), The construction of social judgments 249– 275 Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle ScholarTownsend, J. T., Ashby, F. G.(1983). The stochastic modeling of elementary psychological processes. New York: Cambridge University Press. First citation in articleGoogle ScholarWilson, T. D., Hodges, S. D.(1992). Attitudes as temporary constructions. In L. L. Martin & A. Tesser (Eds.), The construction of social judgments 37– 65 Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle ScholarWittenbrink, B., Judd, C. M., Park, B.(1997). Evidence for racial prejudice at the implicit level and its relationships with questionnaire measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 262– 274 First citation in articleCrossref Medline, Google ScholarZick, A.(1997). Vorurteile und Rassismus. Eine sozialpsychologische Analyse. [Prejudice and racism: A social psychological analysis]. Münster, Germany: Waxmann. First citation in articleGoogle ScholarFiguresReferencesRelatedDetailsCited byWhether implicit attitudes exist is one question, and whether we can measure individual differences effectively is another23 June 2022 | WIREs Cognitive Science, Vol. 13, No. 5Induced disgust increases negative implicit attitudes towards blood donation2 March 2021 | ISBT Science Series, Vol. 16, No. 2Supramodal neural networks support top‐down processing of social signals19 October 2020 | Human Brain Mapping, Vol. 42, No. 3The Persuasive Route to Sustainable Mobility: Elaboration Likelihood Model and Emotions predict Implicit Attitudes2 January 2019 | Environment and Behavior, Vol. 52, No. 8Death Anxiety and Religious Belief : responses to commentaries8 February 2018 | Religion, Brain & Behavior, Vol. 9, No. 2Implicit evaluation of chocolate and motivational need states interact in predicting chocolate intake in everyday lifeEating Behaviors, Vol. 33Teachers’ and preservice teachers’ stereotypes, attitudes, and spontaneous judgments of male ethnic minority studentsStudies in Educational Evaluation, Vol. 59Explicit and implicit speaker evaluations and their differential attitudinal determinantsLanguage Sciences, Vol. 69When and how do explicit measures of food craving predict implicit food evaluation? A moderated mediation modelFood Quality and Preference, Vol. 66EZ: An Easy Way to Conduct a More Fine-Grained Analysis of Faked and Nonfaked Implicit Association Test (IAT) Data1 February 2018 | The Quantitative Methods for Psychology, Vol. 14, No. 1Persistence of the Uncanny Valley28 April 2018Human Perception of Animacy in Light of the Uncanny Valley Phenomenon31 July 2017 | Perception, Vol. 46, No. 12Gender and student misbehavior: Evidence from implicit and explicit measuresTeaching and Teacher Education, Vol. 67Facial race and sex cues have a comparable influence on emotion recognition in Chinese and Australian participants5 July 2017 | Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, Vol. 79, No. 7Effects of Chocolate Deprivation on Implicit and Explicit Evaluation of Chocolate in High and Low Trait Chocolate Cravers12 September 2017 | Frontiers in Psychology, Vol. 8Bad boys, good girls? Implicit and explicit attitudes toward ethnic minority students among elementary and secondary school teachersStudies in Educational Evaluation, Vol. 53The Affect Misattribution Procedure In Search of Prejudice EffectsSarah Teige-Mocigemba, Manuel Becker, Jeffrey W. Sherman, Regina Reichardt, and Karl Christoph Klauer20 June 2017 | Experimental Psychology, Vol. 64, No. 3Genetic and Environmental Sources of Implicit and Explicit Self-Esteem and Affect: Results from a Genetically Sensitive Multi-group Design12 January 2017 | Behavior Genetics, Vol. 47, No. 2Personality self-concept affects processing of trait adjectives in the self-reference memory paradigmJournal of Research in Personality, Vol. 66Anti-fat bias by professors teaching physical education majors24 July 2016 | European Physical Education Review, Vol. 23, No. 1The Effects of Oncologist Implicit Racial Bias in Racially Discordant Oncology InteractionsJournal of Clinical Oncology, Vol. 34, No. 24Trying to separate the wheat from the chaff: Construct- and faking-related variance on the Implicit Association Test (IAT)21 February 2015 | Behavior Research Methods, Vol. 48, No. 1Explicit but not implicit sexist beliefs predict benevolent and hostile sexist behavior14 July 2015 | European Journal of Social Psychology, Vol. 45, No. 6The role of implicit affective responses and trait self-control in ego resource management22 March 2015 | Motivation and Emotion, Vol. 39, No. 5Implicit attitudes and self‐concepts towards transgression and aggression: Differences between male community and offender adolescents, and associations with psychopathic traits25 March 2014 | Journal of Adolescence, Vol. 37, No. 5The Sequential Model of Future-Oriented Coping and Adjustment to University Life: The Role of Attitudes as Further Evidence29 March 2014 | The Psychological Record, Vol. 64, No. 1CULTURAL ADAPTATION (IM)POSSIBLE? MEASURING PREJUDICE IN JAPAN WITH THE AFFECT MISATTRIBUTION PROCEDUREPSYCHOLOGIA, Vol. 57, No. 3Adjusting Shared Reality: Communicators' Memory Changes As Their Connection with Their Audience ChangesSocial Cognition, Vol. 31, No. 2Assessing bias against overweight individuals among nursing and psychology students: an implicit association test18 September 2012 | Journal of Clinical Nursing, Vol. 21, No. 23-24The Affect Misattribution Task Impacts Future Implicit and Explicit JudgmentsSocial Cognition, Vol. 30, No. 5Racialized perceptions and child neglectChildren and Youth Services Review, Vol. 34, No. 8Explicit versus implicit fat-stigma18 February 2012 | American Journal of Human Biology, Vol. 24, No. 3Multikulturelle Überzeugungen Herkunft oder Überzeugung? Welche Rolle spielen der Migrationshintergrund und multikulturelle Überzeugungen für das Unterrichten von Kindern mit Migrationshintergrund?Axinja Hachfeld, Sascha Schroeder, Yvonne Anders, Adam Hahn, and Mareike Kunter20 April 2012 | Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, Vol. 26, No. 2Direct and indirect assessment of explanatory models of illness14 February 2012 | Transcultural Psychiatry, Vol. 49, No. 1Using behavior-analytic implicit tests to assess sexual interests among normal and sex-offender populations15 March 2012 | Socioaffective Neuroscience & Psychology, Vol. 2, No. 1Intentional Faking of the Single Category Implicit Association Test and the Implicit Association Test1 August 2011 | Psychological Reports, Vol. 109, No. 1Positive Associations Primacy in the IAT A Many-Facet Rasch Measurement AnalysisPasquale Anselmi, Michelangelo Vianello, and Egidio Robusto1 January 2011 | Experimental Psychology, Vol. 58, No. 5An Overview of Implicit Measures of Attitudes: Methods, Mechanisms, Strengths, and LimitationsCommunication Methods and Measures, Vol. 5, No. 3The integrative prejudice framework and different forms of weight prejudice14 March 2011 | Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, Vol. 14, No. 3Sexy But Often Unreliable: The Impact of Unreliability on the Replicability of Experimental Findings With Implicit Measures24 March 2011 | Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Vol. 37, No. 4Exaggeration is Harder Than Understatement, but Practice Makes Perfect! Faking Success in the IATJessica Röhner, Michela Schröder-Abé, and Astrid Schütz1 January 2011 | Experimental Psychology, Vol. 58, No. 6When Best Intentions Aren’t Enough: Helping Medical Students Develop Strategies for Managing Bias about Patients30 March 2010 | Journal of General Internal Medicine, Vol. 25, No. S2Regulatory focus and reliance on implicit preferences in consumption contextsJournal of Consumer Psychology, Vol. 20, No. 2Understanding the role of executive control in the Implicit Association Test: Why flexible people have small IAT effects1 March 2010 | Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, Vol. 63, No. 3Using Community Data to Test the Discriminant Validity of Ethnic/Racial Group IATsIrene V. Blair, Charles M. Judd, Edward P. Havranek, and John F. Steiner26 February 2015 | Zeitschrift für Psychologie / Journal of Psychology, Vol. 218, No. 1Control me or I will control you: Impulses, trait self-control, and the guidance of behaviorJournal of Research in Personality, Vol. 43, No. 5Emotion and colour across languages: implicit associations in Spanish colour terms21 August 2009 | Social Science Information, Vol. 48, No. 3Implicit attitudes towards native and non-native speaker teachersSystem, Vol. 37, No. 1Reliability and validity of the Single-Target IAT (ST-IAT): assessing automatic affect towards multiple attitude objectsEuropean Journal of Social Psychology, Vol. 38, No. 6When impulses take over: Moderated predictive validity of explicit and implicit attitude measures in predicting food choice and consumption behaviour24 December 2010 | British Journal of Social Psychology, Vol. 47, No. 3Can Evaluative Conditioning Change Attitudes toward Mature Brands? New Evidence from the Implicit Association Test1 June 2008 | Journal of Consumer Research, Vol. 35, No. 1Convergent and Predictive Validity of Implicit and Explicit Anxiety Measures as a Function of Specificity Similarity and Content SimilarityTobias Gschwendner, Wilhelm Hofmann, and Manfred Schmitt10 October 2008 | European Journal of Psychological Assessment, Vol. 24, No. 4Implicit and Explicit Attitudes and Interracial Interaction: The Moderating Role of Situationally Available Control Resources1 January 2008 | Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, Vol. 11, No. 1Using PsyScope to conduct IAT experiments on Macintosh computersBehavior Research Methods, Vol. 39, No. 4What Do Implicit Measures Tell Us?: Scrutinizing the Validity of Three Common Assumptions22 June 2016 | Perspectives on Psychological Science, Vol. 2, No. 2Experimental Psychology: A Successful TransitionEdgar Erdfelder15 February 2007 | Experimental Psychology, Vol. 54, No. 1Are “implicit” attitudes unconscious?Consciousness and Cognition, Vol. 15, No. 3L’IAT (Implicit Association Test) ou la mesure des cognitions sociales implicites : Revue critique de la validité et des fondements théoriques des scores qu’il produit3 June 2009 | L’Année psychologique, Vol. 106, No. 02Moderatoren der Konsistenz implizit und explizit erfasster Einstellungen und Persönlichkeitsmerkmale Tobias Gschwendner , Wilhelm Hofmann , and Manfred Schmitt 1 September 2006 | Psychologische Rundschau, Vol. 57, No. 1Synergistic Moderator Effects of Situation and Person Factors of Awareness and Adjustment on the Consistency of Implicit and Explicit AttitudesTobias Gschwendner, Wilhelm Hofmann, and Manfred Schmitt1 January 2006 | Journal of Individual Differences, Vol. 27, No. 1Unresolved problems with the “I”, the “A”, and the “T”: A logical and psychometric critique of the Implicit Association Test (IAT)European Review of Social Psychology, Vol. 17, No. 1A Meta-Analysis on the Correlation Between the Implicit Association Test and Explicit Self-Report Measures2 July 2016 | Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Vol. 31, No. 10Contextual Influences on Implicit Evaluation: A Test of Additive Versus Contrastive Effects of Evaluative Context Stimuli in Affective Priming2 July 2016 | Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Vol. 31, No. 9Patterns and Personality Correlates of Implicit and Explicit Attitudes Toward Christians and MuslimsJournal for the Scientific Study of Religion, Vol. 44, No. 1Measuring Task-Switching Ability in the Implicit Association TestMitja D. Back, Stefan C. Schmukle, and Boris Egloff1 September 2006 | Experimental Psychology, Vol. 52, No. 3What moderates implicit—explicit consistency?European Review of Social Psychology, Vol. 16, No. 1Implicit and Explicit Ethnocentrism: Revisiting the Ideologies of Prejudice2 July 2016 | Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, Vol. 30, No. 10Der Implizite Assoziationstest als Maß automatisch aktivierter Assoziationen: Reichweite und Grenzen Bertram Gawronski and Frederica R. Conrey 1 September 2006 | Psychologische Rundschau, Vol. 55, No. 3On the propositional nature of cognitive consistency: Dissonance changes explicit, but not implicit attitudesJournal of Experimental Social Psychology, Vol. 40, No. 4Individuum oder Gruppe, Exemplar oder Kategorie? Melanie C. Steffens , Julia Lichau , Yvonne Still , Petra Jelenec , Julia Anheuser , N. Kristine Goergens , and Tanja Hülsebusch 5 March 2015 | Zeitschrift für Psychologie / Journal of Psychology, Vol. 212, No. 2RESEARCH: Christian Orthodoxy, Religious Fundamentalism, and Right-Wing Authoritarianism as Predictors of Implicit Racial PrejudiceInternational Journal for the Psychology of Religion, Vol. 14, No. 2On Raising the International Dissemination of German Research Does Changing Publication Language to English Attract Foreign Authors to Publish in a German Basic Psychology Research Journal?Andreas Dinkel, Hendrik Berth, Ada Borkenhagen, and Elmar Brähler1 September 2006 | Experimental Psychology, Vol. 51, No. 4Implicit bias in impression formation: associations influence the construal of individuating information1 January 2003 | European Journal of Social Psychology, Vol. 33, No. 5Die Skala Motivation zu vorurteilsfreiem Verhalten: Psychometrische Eigenschaften und Validität Rainer Banse and Bertram Gawronski 1 January 2003 | Diagnostica, Vol. 49, No. 1Editorial Experimental Psychology: Quo vadis?Karl Christoph Klauer1 September 2006 | Experimental Psychology, Vol. 50, No. 1Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm.1 January 2003 | Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 85, No. 2 Volume 49Issue 3July 2002ISSN: 1618-3169eISSN: 2190-5142 tabs.informationExperimental Psychology (2002), 49, pp. 171-180 https://doi.org/10.1026//1618-3169.49.3.171.© 2002Hogrefe & Huber PublishersKeywordsImplicit Association Testprejudicestereotypesimplicit cognitionvaliditytabs.acknowledgements:I would like to thank Rainer Banse for helpful comments on an earlier version of this article; Iain Glen for stylistic corrections; and Cornelia Eibach and Rebecca Wagner for their help in collecting the data.PDF download

Full Text
Published version (Free)

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call