Abstract

This exploratory study assessed the use of functional Near Infrared Spectroscopy (fNIRS) to examine hemodynamic response patterns during sentence processing. Four groups of participants: monolingual English children, bilingual Chinese-English children, bilingual Chinese-English adults and monolingual English adults were given an agent selection syntactic processing task. Bilingual child participants were classified as simultaneous or sequential bilinguals to examine the impact of first language, age of second-language acquisition (AoL2A), and the length of second language experience on behavioral performance and cortical activation. Participants were asked to select the agent of four types of sentences: subject-verb-object (SVO), passive (PAS), subject-extracted relative clause (SR), and object-extracted relative clause (OR) adopted from the “Whatdunit” task by Montgomery et al. (2016). Semantic cues were removed by using inanimate nouns for agents and patients, which constrained participants to make decisions based on syntactic knowledge. Behavioral results showed greater accuracy for canonical SVO and SR sentence types than for noncanonical OR and PAS sentence types, which aligns with prior studies. Neuroimaging results revealed greater hemodynamic responses to relative clauses (i.e., SR and OR sentences) than to simple sentences (SVO and PAS), especially for Chinese-English bilinguals suggesting first-language transfer influencing sentence processing in English. The effects AoL2A and the length of second language experience showed no significant differences between simultaneous and sequential bilinguals or between bilingual adults and children for identifying the correct agent in each sentence. However, neuroimaging results demonstrated greater hemodynamic responses in right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and left inferior parietal lobule (IPL) in simultaneous bilinguals compared to sequential bilinguals and greater hemodynamic responses in left and right DLPFC and left IPL among bilingual adults. Different behavioral and neural hemodynamic response patterns afford new insights into the effects of syntactic knowledge on sentence processing.

Highlights

  • Bilingualism describes the regular use of more than one language, which has become a worldwide phenomenon (Grosjean, 2008)

  • Because relative clauses in Chinese tend to be shorter, less complex, and follow the head-final model as compared with those in English (Lin, 2011), we expected that bilingual groups would evidence more neural recruitment of areas related to cognitive executive function (i.e., dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC)) for subject-extracted relative clause (SR) and object-extracted relative clause (OR) sentence types

  • In right DLPFC and left inferior frontal cortex (IFC), bilingual children evidenced larger area under the curve (AUC) HbO values associated with more neural activation for OR sentences as compared with SVO and PAS sentences

Read more

Summary

Introduction

Bilingualism describes the regular use of more than one language, which has become a worldwide phenomenon (Grosjean, 2008). The appeal of bilingualism and its possible benefits have fostered increased attention from a variety of audiences. Over the past two decades, research using newer neuroimaging methods to study language processes has increased dramatically and the question of how bilinguals process a second language (L2) has become a central issue (Yokoyama et al, 2006; Buchweitz et al, 2012). It has been proposed that bilinguals process an L2 through the same mechanism underlying first-language (L1) processing (Abutalebi, 2008; Waldron and Hernandez, 2013). It remains largely unknown as to how bilinguals process an L2 that differs markedly from their L1. Neuroscience research methods can target such syntactical variations to better understand how bilingual brains process different languages at the sentence level

Methods
Results
Discussion
Conclusion

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.