Abstract

A common and disabling consequence of stroke is the difficulty in processing contralesional space (i.e., hemispatial neglect). According to paper-and-pencil tests, neglect remits or stabilizes in severity within a few months after a brain injury. This arbitrary temporal limit, however, is at odds with neglect’s well-known dependency on task-sensitivity. The present study tested the hypothesis that the putative early resolution of neglect might be due to the insensitivity of testing methods rather than to the lack of spontaneous recovery at later stages. A right hemisphere stroke patient was studied longitudinally for 3 years. According to paper-and-pencil tests the patient showed no symptom of hemispatial neglect 1 month post stroke. Awareness of spatially lateralized visual targets was then assessed by means of computer-based single- and dual-tasks requiring an additional top-down deployment of attention for the parallel processing of visual or auditory stimuli. Errorless performance at computer-based tasks was reached at month 12 and maintained until month 29 after stroke. A bottom-up manipulation was then implemented by reducing target diameter. Following this change, more than 50% of contralesional targets were omitted, mostly under dual-tasking. At months 40 and 41 the same task revealed a significant (but not complete) reduction in the number of contralesional omissions. Ipsilesional targets were, in contrast, still errorless detected. The coupling of a bottom-up (target change) and a top-down (dual-tasking) manipulation revealed the presence of a long-lasting spontaneous recovery from contralesional spatial awareness deficits. In contrast, neither manipulation was effective when implemented separately. After having excluded the potential confound of practice effects, it was concluded that not only the presence but also the time course of hemispatial neglect strongly depends on the degree of attentional engagement required by the task.

Highlights

  • A common and disabling consequence of stroke is the difficulty in processing contralesional space

  • According to paper-and-pencil tests the patient showed no symptom of hemispatial neglect 1 month post stroke

  • The level of attentional engagement required by the task and thepossibility to actively compensate for the spatial attention deficits are major determinants of neglect (Bartolomeo, 1997; Rengachary et al, 2009; Bonato et al, 2010; Bonato and Deouell, 2013; van Kessel et al, 2013)

Read more

Summary

Introduction

A common and disabling consequence of stroke is the difficulty in processing contralesional space (i.e., hemispatial neglect). The coupling of a bottom-up (target change) and a top-down (dual-tasking) manipulation revealed the presence of a long-lasting spontaneous recovery from contralesional spatial awareness deficits. The dependence of contralesional hemispace deficits on task difficulty clearly depicts neglect as a continuous rather than dichotomous disorder According to this view, some patients with non-pathological scores on tests might suffer mild neglect which goes undetected by standard methods. The present study reports on a right hemisphere stroke patient whose (apparent) recovery from visuospatial deficits during the post-acute phase, assessed with both standard tests and computerized tasks, has already been described in detail elsewhere (Bonato et al, 2012). Even considering research performed on earlier stages (e.g., few weeks to months post injury), to my knowledge, there is no study that took practice effects into account when the methods involved repeated administrations of paper-and-pencil tests such as drawing and cancellation. Computer-based tasks have the potential to unveil the presence of spatial neglect even several years post injury (Farnè et al, 2004; Deouell et al, 2005; Bonato, 2012)

Methods
Results
Discussion
Conclusion
Full Text
Paper version not known

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.