Abstract

We typically slow down after committing an error, an effect termed post-error slowing (PES). Traditionally, PES has been calculated by subtracting post-correct from post-error RTs. Dutilh et al. (Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 56(3), 208-216, 2012), however, showed PES values calculated in this way are potentially biased. Therefore, they proposed to compute robust PES scores by subtracting pre-error RTs from post-error RTs. Based on data from a large-scale study using the flanker task, we show that both traditional and robust PES estimates can be biased. The source of the bias are differential imbalances in the percentage of congruent vs. incongruent post-correct, pre-error, and post-error trials. Specifically, we found that post-correct, pre-error, and post-error trials were more likely to be congruent than incongruent, with the size of the imbalance depending on the trial type as well as the length of the response-stimulus interval (RSI). In our study, for trials preceded by a 700-ms RSI, the percentages of congruent trials were 62% for post-correct trials, 66% for pre-error trials, and 56% for post-error trials. Relative to unbiased estimates, these imbalances inflated traditional PES estimates by 37% (9 ms) and robust PES estimates by 42% (16 ms) when individual-participant means were calculated. When individual-participant medians were calculated, the biases were even more pronounced (40% and 50% inflation, respectively). To obtain unbiased PES scores for interference tasks, we propose to compute unweighted individual-participant means by initially calculating mean RTs for congruent and incongruent trials separately, before averaging congruent and incongruent mean RTs to calculate means for post-correct, pre-error and post-error trials.

Highlights

  • For successful goal-directed behavior, it is important to adjust our actions when they are not going according to plan, for instance, when we have committed an error

  • A repeated-measures ANOVA showed that this imbalance was significant: There was a main effect of congruency, F(1, 352) = 1077, p < .001, η2p = 0.75, and an interaction of congruency x response-stimulus interval (RSI), F(1, 352) = 175, p < .001, η2p = 0.33

  • A repeated-measures ANOVA showed that this imbalance was significant: There was a main effect of congruency, F(1, 352) = 3653, p < .001, η2p = 0.91, and an interaction of congruency x RSI, F(1, 352) = 1299, p < .001, η2p = 0.79

Read more

Summary

Introduction

For successful goal-directed behavior, it is important to adjust our actions when they are not going according to plan, for instance, when we have committed an error. Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Leipzig, Germany 5 Department of Education and Psychology, Free University, Berlin, Germany neural and behavioral post-error adjustments (Danielmeier & Ullsperger, 2011; Ullsperger et al, 2014), with post-error slowing (PES), a slowing of reaction times after errors, being the most frequently reported behavioral modulation. It has recently been shown that smaller PES effects in the lab are associated with more self-control failures in real life (Kronke et al, 2018).

Methods
Results
Conclusion
Full Text
Published version (Free)

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call