Abstract

Selective spatial attention is a crucial cognitive process that guides us to the behaviorally relevant objects in a complex visual world by using exploratory eye movements. The spatial location of objects, their (bottom-up) saliency and (top-down) relevance is assumed to be encoded in one “attentional priority map” in the brain, using different egocentric (eye-, head- and trunk-centered) spatial reference frames. In patients with hemispatial neglect, this map is supposed to be imbalanced, leading to a spatially biased exploration of the visual environment. As a proof of concept, we altered the visual saliency (and thereby attentional priority) of objects in a naturalistic scene along a left-right spatial gradient and investigated whether this can induce a bias in the exploratory eye movements of healthy humans (n = 28; all right-handed; mean age: 23 years, range 19–48). We developed a computerized mask, using high-end “gaze-contingent display (GCD)” technology, that immediately and continuously reduced the saliency of objects on the left—“left” with respect to the head (body-centered) and the current position on the retina (eye-centered). In both experimental conditions, task-free viewing and goal-driven visual search, this modification induced a mild but significant bias in visual exploration similar to hemispatial neglect. Accordingly, global eye movement parameters changed (reduced number and increased duration of fixations) and the spatial distribution of fixations indicated an attentional bias towards the right (rightward shift of first orienting, fixations favoring the scene’s outmost right over left). Our results support the concept of an attentional priority map in the brain as an interface between perception and behavior and as one pathophysiological ground of hemispatial neglect.

Highlights

  • Selective visuospatial attention is a crucial cognitive process that enables us to detect the behaviorally relevant object(s) among all the other objects in a complex visual world (Desimone and Duncan, 1995; Fecteau and Munoz, 2006; Moore and Zirnsak, 2017)

  • In order to investigate the impact of this salience modification on both types of visual exploration, i.e., stimulus-driven free viewing and goal-driven visual search, we introduced two different task instructions

  • Despite the dynamically changing position of objects on the retina, we thereby created a permanently reduced sensory input from the left hemispace. This led to a weaker representation of left-hemispace objects in the brain’s attentional priority map and entailed an enduring disadvantage during the competition for attention which made them less likely to be attended during visual exploration (Pouget and Driver, 2000; Serences and Yantis, 2007; Ptak and Fellrath, 2013)

Read more

Summary

Introduction

Selective visuospatial attention is a crucial cognitive process that enables us to detect the behaviorally relevant object(s) among all the other objects in a complex visual world (Desimone and Duncan, 1995; Fecteau and Munoz, 2006; Moore and Zirnsak, 2017). Simulating Neglect via Gaze-Contingent Displays ‘‘attentional priority,’’ a combination of salience and relevance, will be selected in a winner-takes-all fashion (Itti and Koch, 2001; Fecteau and Munoz, 2006; Serences and Yantis, 2007; Bisley and Goldberg, 2010). The favored candidate locus is the parietal lobe containing multisensory neurons able to integrate stimulus-driven (bottom-up) and goal-driven (top-down) information into one topographical map of objects in space (Goldberg et al, 2006; Ipata et al, 2009; Bisley and Goldberg, 2010; Ptak and Fellrath, 2013). There is evidence from fMRI studies (Jerde and Curtis, 2013) that an area in the posterior intraparietal sulcus and an area in the superior precentral sulcus are the most probable candidates for priority maps of space in human cerebral cortex and are proposed to be the human homologs of monkeys’ frontal eye field and lateral intraparietal area

Objectives
Methods
Results
Discussion
Conclusion
Full Text
Paper version not known

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.