Abstract

We wish to answer this question: If you observe a ‘significant’ p-value after doing a single unbiased experiment, what is the probability that your result is a false positive? The weak evidence provided by p-values between 0.01 and 0.05 is explored by exact calculations of false positive risks. When you observe p = 0.05, the odds in favour of there being a real effect (given by the likelihood ratio) are about 3 : 1. This is far weaker evidence than the odds of 19 to 1 that might, wrongly, be inferred from the p-value. And if you want to limit the false positive risk to 5%, you would have to assume that you were 87% sure that there was a real effect before the experiment was done. If you observe p = 0.001 in a well-powered experiment, it gives a likelihood ratio of almost 100 : 1 odds on there being a real effect. That would usually be regarded as conclusive. But the false positive risk would still be 8% if the prior probability of a real effect were only 0.1. And, in this case, if you wanted to achieve a false positive risk of 5% you would need to observe p = 0.00045. It is recommended that the terms ‘significant’ and ‘non-significant’ should never be used. Rather, p-values should be supplemented by specifying the prior probability that would be needed to produce a specified (e.g. 5%) false positive risk. It may also be helpful to specify the minimum false positive risk associated with the observed p-value. Despite decades of warnings, many areas of science still insist on labelling a result of p < 0.05 as ‘statistically significant’. This practice must contribute to the lack of reproducibility in some areas of science. This is before you get to the many other well-known problems, like multiple comparisons, lack of randomization and p-hacking. Precise inductive inference is impossible and replication is the only way to be sure. Science is endangered by statistical misunderstanding, and by senior people who impose perverse incentives on scientists.

Highlights

  • The major point of this paper is that the test of significance does not provide the information concerning psychological phenomena characteristically attributed to it; and that, a great deal of mischief has been associated with its use

  • The fact that we hardly ever have a valid value for the prior probability means that it is impossible to calculate the false positive risk

  • It is often said that p-values exaggerate the evidence against the null hypothesis, this is not strictly true

Read more

Summary

Introduction

The major point of this paper is that the test of significance does not provide the information concerning psychological phenomena characteristically attributed to it; and that, a great deal of mischief has been associated with its use. What you want to know is when a statistical test of significance comes out positive, what the probability is that you have a false positive, i.e. there is no real effect and the results have occurred by chance. This probability is defined here as the false positive risk (FPR). It is assumed throughout this paper that we wish to test a precise hypothesis, e.g. that the effect size is zero (though it makes little difference if we allow a narrow band around zero [3,4]) The reasonableness of this approach is justified in appendix A1. Before getting to results it will be helpful to clarify the ideas that will be used

Definition of terms
Which interpretation is better: ‘p-less-than’ or ‘p-equals’?
Simulation versus exact calculation
Likelihood ratios
Observed likelihood ratios
False positive risk as function of sample size
The reverse Bayesian argument
Discussion
Conclusion and what should be done?
The point null hypothesis
Calculation of likelihood ratios and false positive risks
The calculation of the prior probability: the reverse Bayesian approach
How to do the calculations
Findings
Bayesian estimation in single molecule kinetics
Full Text
Paper version not known

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.