Abstract
The existence of consistent individual differences in behavior has been shown in a number of species, and several studies have found observable sex differences in these behaviors, yet their evolutionary implications remain unclear. Understanding the evolutionary dynamics of behavioral traits requires knowledge of their genetic architectures and whether this architecture differs between the sexes. We conducted a quantitative genetic study in a sexually size‐dimorphic spider, Larinioides sclopetarius, which exhibits sex differences in adult lifestyles. We observed pedigreed spiders for aggression, activity, exploration, and boldness and used animal models to disentangle genetic and environmental influences on these behaviors. We detected trends toward (i) higher additive genetic variances in aggression, activity, and exploration in males than females, and (ii) difference in variances due to common environment/maternal effects, permanent environment and residual variance in aggression and activity with the first two variances being higher in males for both behaviors. We found no sex differences in the amount of genetic and environmental variance in boldness. The mean heritability estimates of aggression, activity, exploration, and boldness range from 0.039 to 0.222 with no sizeable differences between females and males. We note that the credible intervals of the estimates are large, implying a high degree of uncertainty, which disallow a robust conclusion of sex differences in the quantitative genetic estimates. However, the observed estimates suggest that sex differences in the quantitative genetic architecture of the behaviors cannot be ruled out. Notably, the present study suggests that genetic underpinnings of behaviors may differ between sexes and it thus underscores the importance of taking sex differences into account in quantitative genetic studies.
Highlights
Individuals commonly differ in behaviors that are consistent over time and/or context, that is, personality traits, coping styles, or temperament (Gosling, 2001; Koolhaas et al, 1999; Réale et al, 2007)
We detected trends toward (i) higher additive genetic variances in aggression, activity, and exploration in males than females, and (ii) difference in variances due to common environment/maternal effects, permanent environment and residual variance in aggression and activity with the first two variances being higher in males for both behaviors
We note that the credible intervals of the estimates are large, implying a high degree of uncertainty, which disallow a robust conclusion of sex differences in the quantitative genetic estimates
Summary
Individuals commonly differ in behaviors that are consistent over time and/or context, that is, personality traits, coping styles, or temperament (Gosling, 2001; Koolhaas et al, 1999; Réale et al, 2007). We calculate two mean-standardized evolvability measures, the coefficient of additive genetic variation and its square, as well as the coefficient of common environment/maternal effect variation, permanent environmental effect variation, and the coefficient of residual variation These values enable the comparison of components of phenotypic variance and evolvability among different traits, sex, and taxa (Garcia-Gonzalez et al, 2012; Hansen et al, 2011; Houle, 1992). Sit-and-wait predators, whereas males cease web building after reaching maturity; adult males wander around in search of mates and feed mostly commensally in female webs Based on these sex differences, we could predict that males exhibit higher mean levels of activity and exploration, but a previous laboratory study on this species failed to support this expectation (Kralj-Fišer & Schneider, 2012). Photograph: Rok Golobinek predict to find some sex differences in the underlying genetic architecture, because males and females seem to differ in their phenotypic optima for aggression
Talk to us
Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have
Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.