Abstract
Aesthetic experience seems both regular and idiosyncratic. On one hand, there are powerful regularities in what we tend to find attractive versus unattractive (e.g., beaches versus mud puddles).1-4 On the other hand, our tastes also vary dramatically from person to person:5-8 what one of us finds beautiful, another might find distasteful. What is the nature of such differences? They may in part be arbitrary-e.g., reflecting specific past judgments (such as liking red towels over blue ones because they were once cheaper). However, they may also in part be systematic-reflecting deeper differences in perception and/or cognition. We assessed the systematicity of aesthetic taste by exploring its typicality for the first time across seeing and hearing. Observers rated the aesthetic appeal of ordinary scenes and objects (e.g., beaches, buildings, and books) and environmental sounds (e.g., doorbells, dripping, and dialtones). We then measured "taste typicality" (separately for each modality) in terms of the similarity between each individual's aesthetic preferences and the population's average. The data revealed two primary patterns. First, taste typicality was not arbitrary but rather was correlated to a moderate degree across seeing and hearing: people who have typical taste for images also tend to have typical taste for sounds. Second, taste typicality captured most of the explainable variance in people's impressions, showing that it is the primary dimension along which aesthetic tastes systematically vary.
Highlights
We explore the nature of individual aesthetic tastes by asking how different domains of aesthetic taste relate to each other
If people have independent aesthetic tastes in different tion sounds: 33.9%; replication all: 39.6%), and the second prinmodalities, we would expect no relationship between the cipal component never accounted for more than 7.5% additional two scores
This study explored how people’s aesthetic tastes for ordinary visual stimuli relate to those for ordinary environmental sounds
Summary
Past studies have compared individuals’ tastes to some sort of expert standard.[9–12] Here, we correlate individuals’ preferences to the average preferences of the population they come from—seeking to determine not how ‘‘correct’’ or sophisticated those preferences are but how typical they are. Where past studies have computed such correlations[6,8] to assess overall consensus, here we use them to measure individual aesthetic tastes. Past studies of individual aesthetic preferences have often used specialized stimuli, such as artwork,[13–15] music,[16] architecture,[13] cars,[17] or abstract visual shapes.[18–20]. Our experiments do not attempt to address the heights of aesthetic beauty,[21–23] but they instead capture the type of aesthetic impressions that permeate our everyday lives. Such ‘‘ordinary’’ stimuli have been explored in empirical aesthetics more generally,[22,2]
Published Version (Free)
Talk to us
Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have
Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.