Abstract

Simple SummaryCompanion animal-directed speech is a current topic of research, interesting due to its similarity to infant-directed speech. Dog owners seem to almost subconsciously use this high-pitched and repetitive way of speaking, slightly adapted, for dogs. The aim of this study was to investigate dog-directed speech in different contexts and examine whether owner personality and relationship quality affect it. We found that owners’ personality and gender affect their dog-directed speech. The majority of the modifications of dog-directed speech could be explained by a differential use of voice pitch and range. Our study supports the idea that voice pitch was used to communicate affect, whereas pitch range was used as an attention-getting strategy. Based on our results, we conclude that dog-directed speech is adjusted depending on context, gender, and personality. Societal value in this study consists of its contribution to basic knowledge of how we talk to animals, which may help in preventing accidents (e.g., dog bites) as well as improving animal training.Companion animal-directed speech (CADS) has previously been investigated in comparison to infant-directed speech and adult-directed speech. To investigate the influence of owner caregiving, attachment pattern, and personality on CADS, we used the Ainsworth strange situation procedure. It allowed us to assess voice source parameters of CADS across different contexts. We extracted speech parameters (voicing duration, voice pitch, pitch range, and jitter) from 53 dog owners recorded during the procedure. We found that owner personality and gender but not caregiving/attachment behavior affect their voice’s pitch, range, and jitter during CADS. Further, we found a differential and context-specific modification of pitch and range, consistent with the idea that pitch communicates affect, whereas range is more of an attention-getting device. This differential usage, and the increased pitch, emphasize and support the parallels described between CADS and infant-directed speech. For the first time, we also show the effect of personality on CADS and lay the basis for including jitter as a potentially useful measure in CADS.

Highlights

  • It has long been known that the human voice carries substantial cues to the emotional state of the owner, completely independent of any verbal or linguistic content [1]

  • We found that owner personality and gender but not caregiving/attachment behavior affect their voice’s pitch, range, and jitter during Companion animal-directed speech (CADS)

  • We focused attention on source characteristics, voice fundamental frequency (f0, often colloquially termed “voice pitch”), which has reliably been found to vary with emotional state, though often in individual-specific manners [3,4]

Read more

Summary

Introduction

It has long been known that the human voice carries substantial cues to the emotional state of the owner, completely independent of any verbal or linguistic content [1]. The specific forms of information available in companion animal-directed speech (CADS) remain understudied. We analyzed dog-directed speech in different situations to gain insight into this question. We focused attention on source characteristics, voice fundamental frequency (f0, often colloquially termed “voice pitch”), which has reliably been found to vary with emotional state, though often in individual-specific manners [3,4]. Based on this previous research, we hypothesized that voice pitch variables would increase in increasingly arousing situations. We had only a weak a priori prediction that jitter should increase with arousal, perhaps in individual- or personality-specific ways

Objectives
Methods
Results
Discussion
Conclusion
Full Text
Published version (Free)

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call