Abstract

Children’s reasoning on food properties and health relationships can contribute to healthier food choices. Food properties can either be positive (“gives strength”) or negative (“gives nausea”). One of the main challenges in public health is to foster children’s dietary variety, which contributes to a normal and healthy development. To face this challenge, it is essential to investigate how children generalize these positive and negative properties to other foods, including familiar and unfamiliar ones. In the present experiment, we hypothesized that children might rely on cues of food processing (e.g., signs of human intervention such as slicing) to convey information about item edibility. Furthermore, capitalizing on previous results showing that food rejections (i.e., food neophobia and picky eating) are a significant source of inter-individual variability to children’s inferences in the food domain, we followed an individual approach. We expected that children would generalize the positive properties to familiar foods and, in contrast, that they would generalize more often the negative properties to unfamiliar foods. However, we expected that children would generalize more positive and less negative properties to unfamiliar sliced foods than to whole unfamiliar foods. Finally, we expected that children displaying higher levels of food rejections would generalize more negative properties than children displaying lower levels of food rejections. One-hundred and twenty-six children, aged 3–6 years, performed an induction task in which they had to generalize positive or negative health-related properties to familiar or unfamiliar foods, whole or sliced. We measured children’s probability of generalization for positive and negative properties. The children’s food rejection score was assessed on a standardized scale. Results indicated that children evaluated positively familiar foods (regardless of processing), whereas they tend to view unfamiliar food negatively. In contrast, children were at chance for processed unfamiliar foods. Furthermore, children displaying higher levels of food rejections were more likely to generalize the negative properties to all kinds of foods than children displaying lower levels of food rejections. These findings entitle us to hypothesize that knowledge-based food education programs should take into account the valence of the properties taught to children, as well as the state of processing of the food presented. Furthermore, one should take children’s interindividual differences into account because they influence how the knowledge gained through these programs may be generalized.

Highlights

  • Dietary variety is needed for normal and healthy child development (Nicklaus, 2009; Nyaradi et al, 2013)

  • Nutrient deficiency is of particular concern as dietary variety may protect against long-term chronic diseases (Power and Parsons, 2000; Tucker et al, 2006; Zappalla, 2010)

  • The types of food presentations could influence the way children reason about foods and their properties. We addressed these questions from an individual difference perspective by exploring the possible role of food rejection dispositions in children’s induction within the domain of food categories

Read more

Summary

INTRODUCTION

Dietary variety is needed for normal and healthy child development (Nicklaus, 2009; Nyaradi et al, 2013). Investigating neophobic and picky children’s reasoning on food properties for inferences about the negative health-related effects of eating is of both theoretical and practical importance If these children are more sensitive to food’s risks, they might generalize this information to more foods than their neophobic, or less fussy, counterparts. Most of the previous studies focused on children’s inductive reasoning on foods with familiar or unfamiliar foods and did not directly compare them They did not manipulate food processing states (whole, sliced, or cooked), which has been shown to influence edibility judgments and food preferences, at least in adults. We asked children to generalize a positive or negative property associated with a training familiar fruit or vegetable, to other foods from the same taxonomic category as the training, familiar or unfamiliar, and whole or sliced. We expect that neophobic children will generalize more negative properties to unfamiliar foods compared to their neophilic counterparts

Participants
Design
Procedure
RESULTS
DISCUSSION
Findings
CONCLUSION
ETHICS STATEMENT
Full Text
Published version (Free)

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call