Abstract
When experiencing personal distress, people usually expect their romantic partner to be supportive. However, when put in a situation to provide support, people may at times (still) be struggling with issues of their own. This interdependent nature of dyadic coping interactions as well as potential spillover effects is mirrored in the state-of-the-art research method to behaviorally assess couple’s dyadic coping processes. This paradigm typically includes two videotaped 8-min dyadic coping conversations in which partners swap roles as sharer and support provider. Little is known about how such dyadic coping interactions may feed back into one another, impacting the motivation and ability to be a responsive support provider. In three behavioral studies, we examined how sharers’ experiences may spill over to affect their own support provision in a subsequent dyadic coping interaction. We hypothesized that the extent to which sharers perceive their partner as responsive to their self-disclosure increases the quality of their own subsequent support provision (Hypothesis 1), whereas sharers’ lingering negative affect reduces the quality of their own subsequent support provision (Hypothesis 2). In line with our first hypothesis, perceived partner responsiveness predicted the provision of higher-quality support, though primarily as perceived by the partner. Sharers who perceived their partner to have been more responsive were somewhat more likely to subsequently engage in positive dyadic coping and were rated as more responsive by their partners. Negative dyadic coping behavior was unaffected. Evidence for our second hypothesis was mixed. While lingering negative affect did not affect positive dyadic coping behavior or perceived support, it did increase the chances of negative dyadic coping behavior. However, given the very low occurrences of negative affect and negative dyadic coping, these findings should be interpreted with caution. Taken together, these findings suggest that support interactions may feed back into one another, highlighting the complex and interdependent nature of dyadic coping. The strongest and most consistent findings concerned the spillover effect of perceived partner responsiveness on subsequent perceived support quality, speaking to the key role of believing that one’s partner is responsive to one’s needs in promoting healthy relationship functioning.
Highlights
Imagine coming home after a rough day at work, where your boss unexpectedly just fired one of your favorite colleagues
With regard to the first hypothesis, we assessed the evidence for an effect of perceived partner responsiveness during the first dyadic coping interaction on the three different measures of quality of support provided during the second dyadic coping interaction
There was little evidence for a positive effect of perceived responsiveness (T1) on subsequent positive dyadic coping behavior (T2): Individuals who experienced higher levels of partner responsiveness during the first interaction may or may not have been more likely to engage in positive dyadic coping behavior themselves when they were listening to their partner in the subsequent interaction [BF10 = 3.57; B = 0.07 on the logit scale, 95% credible interval (−0.11, 0.24); Figure 2A]
Summary
Imagine coming home after a rough day at work, where your boss unexpectedly just fired one of your favorite colleagues. One of the core features of close relationships concerns sharing one’s intimate emotional experiences with one’s partner (e.g., Laurenceau et al, 2004) This type of self-disclosure is crucial for fostering intimacy and hinges on the partner’s responsiveness to sharer’s needs (for overviews, see Laurenceau et al, 2004; Reis et al, 2004, 2017; Reis and Gable, 2015). An overlooked issue is how these support interactions may feed back into one another, impacting the quality of support provision This interdependent nature of dyadic coping interactions as well as potential spillover effects is mirrored in the state-of-the-art research method to behaviorally assess couple’s dyadic coping processes. These interactions are typically studied as independent while they likely are not (see Laurenceau et al, 2004; Joseph and Afifi, 2010; Joseph et al, 2016; Leuchtmann et al, 2018)
Talk to us
Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have
Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.