Abstract

Our eyes are in continuous motion. Even when we attempt to fix our gaze, we produce so called “fixational eye movements”, which include microsaccades, drift, and ocular microtremor (OMT). Microsaccades, the largest and fastest type of fixational eye movement, shift the retinal image from several dozen to several hundred photoreceptors and have equivalent physical characteristics to saccades, only on a smaller scale (Martinez-Conde, Otero-Millan & Macknik, 2013). OMT occurs simultaneously with drift and is the smallest of the fixational eye movements (∼1 photoreceptor width, >0.5 arcmin), with dominant frequencies ranging from 70 Hz to 103 Hz (Martinez-Conde, Macknik & Hubel, 2004). Due to OMT’s small amplitude and high frequency, the most accurate and stringent way to record it is the piezoelectric transduction method. Thus, OMT studies are far rarer than those focusing on microsaccades or drift. Here we conducted simultaneous recordings of OMT and microsaccades with a piezoelectric device and a commercial infrared video tracking system. We set out to determine whether OMT could help to restore perceptually faded targets during attempted fixation, and we also wondered whether the piezoelectric sensor could affect the characteristics of microsaccades. Our results showed that microsaccades, but not OMT, counteracted perceptual fading. We moreover found that the piezoelectric sensor affected microsaccades in a complex way, and that the oculomotor system adjusted to the stress brought on by the sensor by adjusting the magnitudes of microsaccades.

Highlights

  • IntroductionMicrosaccades, the largest and fastest type of fixational eye movement, shift the retinal image from several dozen to several hundred photoreceptors and have equivalent physical characteristics to saccades, only on a smaller scale (Martinez-Conde, Macknik & Hubel, 2004; Martinez-Conde et al, 2009; Martinez-Conde, Otero-Millan & Macknik, 2013)

  • We extracted the microsaccadic component of the piezoelectric sensor data using a wavelet denoising technique (Al-Kalbani et al, 2007) and we detected the microsaccades from the EyeLink II data using a modified version of Engbert and Kliegl’s algorithm (Engbert & Kliegl, 2003) (Fig. 1C; see Materials and Methods for details)

  • Because the piezoelectric sensor comes in contact with the eye, we wondered whether its presence might affect microsaccade dynamics, for instance by dampening eye movements during simultaneous piezoelectric and video recordings

Read more

Summary

Introduction

Microsaccades, the largest and fastest type of fixational eye movement, shift the retinal image from several dozen to several hundred photoreceptors and have equivalent physical characteristics to saccades, only on a smaller scale (Martinez-Conde, Macknik & Hubel, 2004; Martinez-Conde et al, 2009; Martinez-Conde, Otero-Millan & Macknik, 2013). Drift is a slow (typically < 2◦/s) curvy motion, resembling a random walk, that occurs between saccades and/or microsaccades (Engbert & Kliegl, 2004). OMT occurs simultaneously with drift and is the smallest of the fixational eye movements (∼1 photoreceptor width,

Methods
Results
Conclusion
Full Text
Paper version not known

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.