Abstract

Rumination, DEFINED AS REPETITIVE, RECURRENT AND UNCONTROLLABLE THINKING, has been implicated in cognitive impairments, particularly dysexecutive function, in people with depressed mood, what is now critical is an examination of the link between rumination and cognitive impairment independent of mood. Rumination’s direct relationship to basic cognitive functions may help explain the stability of rumination, and how it predicts future episodes of depressed mood. Two experiments examined how trait and experimentally induced rumination, and negative mood relate to attention and inhibition at varying levels of cognitive load in non-depressed participants. Study 1 found trait rumination significantly predicted errors of attention when cognitive load was low or high, but not at a medium level. Trait rumination also interacted with frequency of task unrelated thoughts to predict performance. Negative mood significantly predicted errors of inhibition also when the cognitive load was low or high. Study 2 expanded on the first by including two additional measures of executive function. Results were replicated for the first study and showed a ruminative thought style predicted inhibition, reaction time and set shifting in the additional measures.

Highlights

  • There are many ways to conceptualise rumination; a response to depressed mood (Nolen-Hoeksema, Parker, & Larson, 1994; Verhaeghen, Joorman, & Khan, 2005), an emotion regulation strategy (Nolen-Hoeksema, Wisco, & Lyubomirsky, 2008), a form of self-focused attention (Mor & Winquist, 2002), as a process or style of thinking (Brinker & Dozois, 2009) and so on

  • Despite participants being randomly assigned to conditions, there was a significant difference between experimental groups on baseline ruminative thought styles (t = 2.23, p < .05) with the rumination group reporting higher levels of trait ruminative thought than the distraction group (M = 91.06 and 83.24 respectively)

  • All correlations were significant and in the expected direction (Papageorgiou & Siegle, 2003) with GREATER rumination being significantly related to greater negative mood as measured by the PANAS-N and the visual analogue scales

Read more

Summary

Introduction

There are many ways to conceptualise rumination; a response to depressed mood (Nolen-Hoeksema, Parker, & Larson, 1994; Verhaeghen, Joorman, & Khan, 2005), an emotion regulation strategy (Nolen-Hoeksema, Wisco, & Lyubomirsky, 2008), a form of self-focused attention (Mor & Winquist, 2002), as a process or style of thinking (Brinker & Dozois, 2009) and so on These conceptions are not necessarily contradictory, but different ways of considering and examining a complex phenomenon. The stability of ruminative thinking suggests the process may be related to individual differences in basic cognitive function—in particular executive functioning

Methods
Results
Conclusion
Full Text
Paper version not known

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.