Abstract
To the Editor: In epidemiology, overadjustment bias is defined as adjusting for a variable that increases rather than decrease bias, while unnecessary adjustment is referred to as control for a variable that adversely affects precision without introducing bias.1,2 The term of overadjustment bias is used to refer to different scenarios. Here, we propose a unified definition of overadjustment, with four types. Type 1 overadjustment is the classic adjustment for an intermediate variable or a descendant of an intermediate variable (see Table). Schisterman et al1 explained type 1 overadjustment bias in detail. If the total effect of the exposure on the outcome is of interest, adjusting for an intermediate variable, or a downstream proxy of an intermediate variable may lead to overadjustment bias because (in ideal settings) such adjustment results in an estimate of the controlled direct effect instead of the total effect.TABLE 1.: Four Types of Overadjustment BiasType 2 overadjustment is adjustment for a collider or a descendant of a collider.3 Collider bias is defined as a bias that occurs when conditioning on a common effect (or a descendant of a common effect) of two variables, of which one is the exposure or a cause of the exposure, and the other is the outcome or a cause of the outcome.4 Here, we make a distinction between collider restriction bias and collider stratification bias. Collider restriction bias due to restricting to one level of a collider is a form of selection bias; however, collider stratification bias through (for example) the unnecessary inclusion of a collider in a regression model may be best understood as a type of overadjustment bias instead of selection bias. However, additionally blocking another covariate that lies on a backdoor path that is opened by collider adjustment, if possible, will obviate the bias. Type 3 overadjustment is adjustment for an instrumental variable in the presence of unmeasured confounding. An instrumental variable is a cause of the exposure that has no relation to the outcome except through the exposure.5 It has been shown that in a regression model of the outcome on the exposure, adjusting for a strong instrumental variable (more broadly, strong predictors of the exposure), in the presence of unmeasured confounding, has the potential to amplify bias as well as affect precision, which we term an overadjustment bias.6,7 Amplification occurs because, within strata of the exposure, the instrumental variable is associated with the outcome via the unmeasured confounder. Thus, in practice, it is best to exclude pure instrumental variables from covariate control. Type 4 overadjustment is adjustment for a descendant of the outcome when the exposure has a causal effect on the outcome. As shown in the Table, adjusting for a descendant of the outcome D is in fact adjusting for a descendant of the collider, when there is an another cause of outcome D (that is L) that makes the outcome D a collider.8 As a result, it induces a spurious association between E and L and hence between E and D, therefore resulting in biased effect estimates of exposure E on outcome D on risk difference and risk ratio scales. Thus, adjustment for a descendant of the outcome can also be regarded as a special type of collider bias. Here, we summarized four types of covariate adjustment that may induce bias, and, hence, we call these overadjustment biases. Consciously avoiding these types of overadjustment bias, together with adjustment for a sufficient set of confounders (both of which rely on substantive knowledge), is necessary to satisfy the assumption of correct causal model specification in the standard set of identification conditions.9 Correct causal model specification, followed by correct statistical model specification (which determines the functional form and relationships between exposure, covariates, and outcome), along with other standard assumptions (e.g., no measurement error), constitute an important set of sufficient conditions for the identification of causal effects in epidemiology and beyond.10,11 In some circumstances, a variable may play more than one role, and while this is beyond the scope of the present paper, whether or not to adjust for such variable may well become an issue of minimizing overall bias. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The authors thank the anonymous reviewer of this letter for their helpful comments.
Talk to us
Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have
Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.