Abstract

To the Editor: We must admit that Dr. Visscher (2002 [in this issue]) is quite correct and that in our two reports of twins in autism (Greenberg et al. 2001; Betancur et al. 2002) we overlooked the elementary application of Bayes’s rule in this situation, namely: If MZ twin pairs are more likely to be concordant than nontwin pairs, then sampling concordant pairs will produce an excess of MZ twin pairs relative to nontwin pairs. This excess says nothing about the relative strengths of genetic or nongenetic effects in autism, contrary to what we concluded in our papers. However, the points made by Dr. Visscher explain only part of our observations, and they also highlight the sensitivity of the conclusions to the accuracy of the population data. Because twin concordance rates vary from study to study, the issue of increased autism risk to twins is not yet settled. In particular, interpreting the findings from the DZ twins remains problematic. We begin with some calculation issues. First, Visscher’s formulas contain an error, although the error does not affect his conclusions and may even strengthen them. The probability of both members of a sib pair being affected, his P(2 affected), does not equal rp, as he states. (We use his notation of r for the “pairwise concordance rate,” but note that p should represent disease population prevalence, not incidence.) Rather, P(2 affected) is given by Kp, where K is the “recurrence risk” for that particular kind of sib pair (James 1971; Risch 1990). To see why, let us use πi for Visscher’s P(i affected)—that is, the probability that a sib pair has i affected sibs. The standard definition (also used by Visscher) says that the pairwise concordance rate r gives the probability that both sibs are affected, given that at least one is affected—that is, r≡π2/(π2+π1). In contrast, the recurrence risk K is defined as the recurrence risk to the sib of an affected individual—that is, P(sib #2 is affected|sib #1 is affected), which can be written as π2/P(randomindividualisaffected)=π2/p. Since K=π2/p, (The recurrence risk K is the same as the “probandwise concordance rate” for twins [Smith 1974]; also see Wickramaratne and Hodge 2001). K is always ⩾r, approaching twice r when both are small: We now recalculate Visscher’s f*MZ, defined as P(MZ|2 affected). We incorporate the above correction from equation (1) (i.e., replace r with K), and we use the values for f (the population probability of each type of sib pair, among all sib pairs) from Greenberg et al. (2001): fMZ=.008, fDZ=.016, fS=1-.008-.016=.976. The formula becomes Visscher approximates this quantity by (.008)KMZ/[(.976)KS] and examines the effect on the ratio fMZ/fS, but we prefer to work directly with f*MZ. Combining data from the three epidemiologically based twin studies of autism cited by Folstein and Rosen-Sheidley (2001) (Folstein and Rutter 1977; Steffenburg et al. 1989; Bailey et al. 1995), we note that 25 of 36 MZ twin pairs are concordant. (This figure comes from rMZ=15/25=0.60 in the work by Bailey et al., which includes the Folstein and Rutter data, as well as 10 of 11 concordant MZ pairs in the work by Steffenburg et al.) This yields rMZ=25/36=0.69, which we convert to KMZ=0.82, using equation (2). As for nontwin sib pairs, several studies (August et al. 1981; Piven et al. 1990; Bolton et al. 1994, cited in Lauritsen and Ewald 2001) agree on a sib recurrence risk of ∼3%. Note that this is a recurrence value, so we use it unchanged. Ritvo et al. (1989), in the largest published study of sibs, reported a slightly higher figure of 4.5% among all sibs of the firstborn subject. We will also assume KDZ equals KS for the purposes of these calculations. Using equation (3), we calculate f*MZ as 18% or 13%, for KS=3% or 4.5%, respectively. These predictions agree reasonably closely with the rates of MZ twin pairs observed by us: 10%–14% (Greenberg et al. 2001) and 10%–13% (Betancur et al. 2002). Thus, we agree with Visscher that our observed rates fit right within what would be predicted by the respective MZ twin and sibling recurrence rates. (We have used somewhat different input figures than Visscher, because we went back to the original studies, but the point remains the same.) However, additional questions remain: (1) Although the sib recurrence rates seem to be fairly consistent among studies, that is not true of the published MZ twin concordance rates, which vary widely and depend highly on issues of ascertainment, diagnosis, etc. (Smith 1974). It appears to be more difficult to collect unbiased, clearly diagnosed samples of twin pairs than of nontwin sib pairs. Thus, the KMZ rates are “soft”; if they turned out to be lower than those used here, then the conclusions could be quite different. (2) If the reasoning outlined by Visscher and discussed above completely “explains away” the striking increase in twin pairs among affected sib pairs observed by both our groups, then why has that increase not been observed in other autism data sets as well? Is this phenomenon wholly due to most investigators rejecting MZ twin pairs for their studies, which our two groups did not do? And/or is it due to investigators simply not examining their data sets for excess twins? We do not know the answer, but this situation illustrates the usefulness of tracking how and what kind of families enter a study. (3) Similarly, if the above reasoning explains the increase in twin pairs, why is a similar increase not observed in data from other diseases? In one of our original articles (Greenberg et al. 2001), for example, we had looked at affected sib pairs (ASPs) with insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus (IDDM) for just this reason, to provide a control. A relatively recent study (Kyvik et al. 1995) of IDDM concordance in Danish twins found recurrence risks (probandwise concordances) of KMZ=0.70 and KDZ=0.13 (cumulative age-adjusted). This study was based on 20,888 twin pairs from a population-based nationwide register and was probably more free of ascertainment problems than earlier studies. Sib recurrence risks for IDDM are usually estimated at ∼0.06 (Thomson et al. 1988; Dorman et al. 1995). Inserting these values into equation (3) yields f*MZ=.085, yet, in our control sample of ASPs with IDDM, collected in the same manner as the ASPs with autism, we observed only 13/649=0.02 MZ twin pairs—nothing like the kind of excess predicted by equation (3). In fact, our observed proportion would represent a highly significant deficit relative to what was expected. Moreover, one can also calculate f*DZ—that is, P(DZ|2 affected)—by replacing (.008)KMZ with (.016)KDZ in the numerator of equation (3), yielding f*DZ=0.03. So we should have observed a proportion of 0.03 DZ twins among the IDDM sib pairs, higher than the population rate of .016; yet, we observed again a statistically significant deficit of DZ twin pairs (1/649=0.002). This remains puzzling. On the other hand, a 1992 Finnish study of IDDM twin concordance (Kaprio et al. 1992) found much lower recurrence rates: KMZ=0.23 and KDZ=0.05. Using those values, one would predict observing f*MZ≈0.04 and f*DZ≈0.016 (we set both KDZ and KS to 0.05 for that calculation), which is much closer to what we did observe in the IDDM data. This discrepancy between the two studies, only 3 years apart, highlights again the “softness” of twin concordance rates and the difficulty of drawing firm conclusions from them. An older study by Cahill (1979) had also reported a low KMZ of only 5/28=18%. (4) The DZ twin pairs in the autism data sets are puzzling as well. We did observe increased numbers of these pairs—a striking and significant increase under both narrow and broad diagnostic criteria by Greenberg et al. (2001) and a slight (not significant) increase by Betancur et al. (2002). Yet the literature reports KDZ no higher than KS for autism, so there should have been no increase over population proportions. Visscher mentions the “stoppage” phenomenon as possibly explaining the excess in DZ twins, but we did account for stoppage in the original Greenberg et al. (2001) article. We agree with Dr. Visscher that to provide a definitive answer to these questions will require a population-based twin study examining the prevalence of autism among MZ and DZ twins. One of us (C.G.) is currently undertaking such a study in Sweden, and we hope this will help answer these questions. Moreover, a just-published epidemiological study examined >3.5 million live births in California between 1989 and 1994, of whom 4,381 were diagnosed with what those authors call “full-syndrome” autism (Croen et al. 2002). This study found an increased autism risk associated with multiple births: relative risk was 1.7 (95% CI 1.4–2.0), when adjusted for all other factors considered by the authors. Thus, we feel that whether twinness represents a risk factor for autism is still an open question. However, for the time being, we must agree with Dr. Visscher that the observed proportions of twins in both our autism data sets argue neither for nor against the hypothesis that being a twin is itself a risk factor for autism.

Full Text
Published version (Free)

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call