Abstract

Underwater sound is used by many marine larvae to orient to coastal habitats including backreef, sponge-dominated hardbottom habitat in the Florida Keys (FL, United States)—a particularly “noisy” coastal habitat. However, the distance over which acoustic cues are attractive to settlement-stage larvae is generally unknown. We examined this phenomenon in a region of the Florida Keys where mass sponge die-offs have diminished both underwater soundscapes and larval settlement. The absence of pronounced hardbottom-associated sound over such a large area allowed us to experimentally test in situ the response of fish and invertebrate larvae to broadcasted sounds at different distances from their source. We first measured the signal-to-noise ratio of healthy hardbottom habitat soundscapes broadcast from an underwater speaker at seven distances to determine the maximum range of the signal. Based on those results, larval collectors were then deployed at 10, 100, 500, and 1,000 m from speakers broadcasting sounds recorded at either degraded or healthy hardbottom sites for five consecutive nights during each of three new and full moon periods in summer/fall 2019. Larval settlement onto those collectors was affected by lunar phase and soundscape type, but varied among species. In most cases, the effect was small and not likely to be ecologically significant. The absence of a strong larval settlement response to a sound cue lies in contrast to results from other studies. We suspect that the small (<500 m) radius of the broadcasted soundscapes may have limited the magnitude of the larval response to locally available larvae whose abundance may have been low because the experiment was conducted within a large, relatively quiet seascape. If true, it is possible that planktonic larvae may require a series of acoustic “sign-posts,” perhaps in combination with other cues (e.g., chemical), to successfully orient to distant nursery habitats. Although habitat restoration efforts may be able to restore healthy soundscapes, the typically small size and number of restoration sites may limit the range of the acoustic cue and thus larval attraction to restored habitats.

Highlights

  • Soundscapes, the collection of sounds emanating from landscapes, convey important information about an environment and its biota

  • Because the recordings used in the larval settlement experiment (65–80 dB re 1 μPa) were quieter than the tones used in the transmission loss experiment (115 dB re 1μPa), a curve was fit to the transmission loss results (Figure 4) and we calculated the distance where the signal to noise ratio (SNR) was equal to 2.6 dB, near the limit of detection

  • Larval assemblages differed between moon phases (PERMANOVA: F1,141 = 6.377, R2 = 0.043, p = 0.001) and mean settlement per collector was 1.3 times higher during full moon than new moon

Read more

Summary

Introduction

Soundscapes, the collection of sounds emanating from landscapes, convey important information about an environment and its biota. Marine soundscapes are correlated with environmental (e.g., sea state, habitat depth) and biological parameters (e.g., coral cover, fish diversity and biomass; Piercy et al, 2014; Kaplan et al, 2017), and those acoustic signals are used by pelagic larvae to locate nursery habitats. Sound is different from other settlement cues (i.e., visual or olfactory cues) because it can carry information over long distances and is not affected by currents, turbidity, or light. For sound to be a useful settlement cue, pelagic larvae must be able to detect it far from the source. Many studies demonstrate the use of sound cues by fish and invertebrate larvae, the range at which they can detect and discriminate between cues is unclear

Objectives
Methods
Results
Conclusion
Full Text
Paper version not known

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.