Abstract

Dear Sir: We appreciate Cedernaes and Benedict’s interest in our recent work on eating behaviors in relation to duration of sleep (1). The focus of the letter by Cedernaes and Benedict is metabolic and health concerns due to disruption of circadian eating and sleeping norms in shift work, a promising area of study, which was not the subject of our article. In their summary of findings of our study, Cedernaes and Benedict state that “self-reported eating behavior did not differ between average and long sleepers (ie, ≥9 h).” Our article makes no such assertions. In fact, both tabular and narrative results show that many eating behaviors of long-duration sleepers were similar to those of short-duration sleepers but different from average-duration sleepers (eg, less likely to report breakfast, less energy from main meals, more energy from snacks, etc). These findings are also mentioned in the Discussion. Cedernaes and Benedict also state that we did not examine “whether short sleepers also eat at times of the day when the circadian system is not metabolically adjusted to process ingested nutrients or stimulants such as caffeine (ie, primarily during the night).” Our study addressed this question in at least 3 ways by examination of the following: 1) the percentage of 24-h energy that was reported at or after 2000 h (shown in our Table 2), 2) the number of snack episodes (and % of energy) reported after dinner by dinner reporters (shown in our Table 3), and 3) the clock time of the last reported eating episode of the recalled day (shown in our Table 4). Short-duration sleepers did report modestly higher energy intakes at or after 2000 h and a later time of the last eating episode of the day. Last, we want to emphasize that the exposure in our study was hours of self-reported weekday/workday nighttime sleep. During our preliminary analyses we found employment status (yes or no) to be a correlate of sleep duration as well as of energy intake and eating behaviors. Hence, all reported analyses in the article were adjusted for employment status. It is unlikely that additional control for shift work will alter our findings. Overall, in our analytic sample, 10.87% (95% CI: 10.13%, 11.65%) of adult Americans reported evening/night/rotating work schedules. The prevalences (sample weighted %) of evening, night, and rotating work schedules in short-, average-, and long-duration sleepers were 13.45% (12.19%, 14.82%), 9.20% (8.34%, 10.14%), and 10.78% (7.92%, 14.51%), respectively. The multiple variable–adjusted mean hours of sleep reported by regular day schedule reporters (6.82 h; 95% CI: 6.77, 6.87 h) were not significantly different from those of evening/night/rotating schedule reporters (6.76 h; 95% CI: 6.65, 6.88 h). Mean hours of sleep reported by nonemployed Americans, however, were higher at 7.01 h (95% CI: 6.94, 7.08 h) and significantly different from those who worked day or evening/night/rotating schedules. Interestingly, in the NHANES 2007–2008, the metabolic profiles (eg, BMI, waist circumference, serum triglycerides, LDL cholesterol, and glycated hemoglobin) of Americans reporting evening/night/rotating work schedules did not differ from those of regular day shift workers (2). Nevertheless, to address the concern about possible confounding of sleep duration and eating behavior association by shift work, we conducted further sensitivity analyses. We repeated analyses for key eating behavior outcomes after exclusion of evening/night/rotating shift reporters from the analytic sample and after addition of a variable to indicate evening/night/rotating schedule to the regression models. With both approaches, as shown in Table 1, the substantive findings of our study were unchanged. TABLE 1 Adjusted mean (95% CI) intakes of total energy, percentages of 24-h energy from main and non–main meals at or after 2000 h, and clock times of first and last episodes of the recalled day reported by adult Americans by categories of weekday/workday ... The review by Lowden et al (3), also cited by Cedernaes and Benedict, found the available evidence on the association of shift work and eating to be “complex” and “contradictory.” Thus, we look forward to more research on norms of circadian rhythms of eating and sleeping behaviors and individual adaptations to dyssynchrony resulting from constraints of work and leisure in modern societies. We agree with Cedernaes and Benedict about the value of population surveys (such as the NHANES) to provide useful information about eating behaviors of shift workers. However, given the cross-sectional nature of such surveys, the complexity of eating and physical activity behaviors, and measurement errors in self-reports of behaviors of free-living humans, we caution against causal inferences about “energy surplus” and “metabolic consequences.”

Full Text
Published version (Free)

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call