Abstract

Recency Effects in Category Learning are Dynamic and Adaptive matt jones (mattj@umich.edu) Department of Psychology, The University of Michigan 525 E. University Ave, Ann Arbor, MI 48109-1109 USA Winston R. Sieck (sieck.3@osu.edu) Department of Psychology, The Ohio State University 1827 Neil Ave, Columbus, OH 43210 USA Recency Effects (REs) have been well established in both memory and probability learning paradigms. However, these effects have received relatively little attention, and virtually no empirical investigation, in research on multiple- cue category learning. Some category learning models do make specific a priori predictions regarding REs. In particular, simple, static REs arise naturally in models incorporating the δ-rule, an error- driven learning mechanism (Gluck & Bower, 1988; Kruschke, 1992). Specifically, the δ-rule implies that responses are based on a weighted average of past cases, with recent cases receiving more weight. Weights drop at an exponential rate with temporal distance. Exemplar models make no a priori predictions regarding RE, but can mimic δ-rule predictions by explicitly incorporating exponential trace decay (Nosofsky, Kruschke, & McKinley, Both of these approaches predict RE to be unresponsive to characteristics of the task environment. The present research contrasts this assumption with the alternative possibility that people explicitly test hypotheses concerning the predictive validity of recent outcomes. The latter idea implies that the magnitude of recency effects will adapt to the level of autocorrelation in the category sequence. Method Participants (n=100) engaged in a simulated medical diagnosis task in which they assigned each of a series of 150 hypothetical patients to one of two fictitious diseases based on the presence or absence of 3 symptoms, with outcome feedback given after each trial. The experiment consisted of three conditions based on the manner of autocorrelation present in the sequence of disease outcomes. In the positive condition, each patient’s disease matched that of the previous patient with 70% probability; in the negative condition that probability was 30%. Trials in the control condition were independently sampled (i.e., a 50% transition probability). For each subject, the diseases were first randomly generated according to the condition, and then the symptoms were randomly generated with probabilities dependent on disease outcomes. Results A logistic regression model was fitted to the data from each subject to determine the degree of influence of both present and past information on that subject’s responses. The predictors in this model were the three symptoms, the disease of the previous patient (Disease n-1 ), the similarity of the previous patient’s symptom profile with that of the n present patient (C n-1 , given as the number of matching minus the number of mismatching symptom dimensions), and the interaction Disease n-1 x C n n-1. The regression coefficient for Disease n-1 was significantly higher for subjects in the Positive condition as compared to Control, indicating a heightened recency effect (Fig. 1). The reverse effect was not found for the Negative condition, despite its logical symmetry with the Positive condition. n In addition, the coefficient for Disease n-1 x C n-1 was significantly positive in all 3 conditions, showing that cue commonality moderates RE. Positive Negative Control Disease n-1 Disease n-1 x C n n-1 Figure 1: Subjects’ use of past information, by condition Discussion Reliance on recent outcomes increased dramatically when those outcomes were positively related to the current case. No corresponding effect was found for negatively related recent outcomes. In fact, no overall RE was found in the negative and control conditions. These effects are not anticipated by the standard proposal that impact of outcomes simply decays exponentially with time. People appear to assess the predictive validity of recent events, but are nevertheless biased towards expecting a positive relationship. References Gluck, M. A., & Bower, G. H. (1988). Evaluating an adaptive network model of human learning. Journal of Memory & Language, 27, 166-195. Kruschke, J. K. (1992). ALCOVE: An exemplar-based connectionist model of category learning. Psychological Review, 99, 22-44. Nosofsky, R. M., Kruschke, J. K., & McKinley, S. C. Combining exemplar-based category representations and connectionist learning rules. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 18, 211-233.

Full Text
Paper version not known

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.