Abstract

Previous research demonstrates that tufted capuchin monkeys use terrestrial predator alarm calls in a functionally deceptive manner to distract conspecifics when feeding on contestable resources, although the success of this tactic is limited because listeners frequently ignore these calls when given in such situations. While this decreased response rate is suggestive of a counterstrategy to deception by receivers, the proximate factors underpinning the behavior are unclear. The current study aims to test if the decreased response rate to alarm calls in competitive contexts is better explained by the perception of subtle acoustic differences between predator-elicited and deceptive false alarms, or by receivers varying their responses based on the context in which the signal is received. This was tested by first examining the acoustic structure of predator-elicited and deceptive false alarms for any potentially perceptible acoustic differences, and second by comparing the responses of capuchins to playbacks of each of predator-elicited and false alarms, played back in noncompetitive contexts. The results indicate that deceptive false alarms and predator-elicited alarms show, at best, minimal acoustic differences based on the structural features measured. Likewise, playbacks of deceptive false alarms elicited antipredator reactions at the same rate as did predator-elicited alarms, although there was a nonsignificant tendency for false alarms to be more likely to elicit escape reactions. The lack of robust acoustic differences together with the high response rate to false alarms in noncompetitive contexts suggests that the context in which the signal is received best explains receiver responses. It remains unclear, however, if listeners ascribe different meanings to the calls based on context, or if they generally ignore all signals in competitive contexts. Whether or not the decreased response rate of receivers directly stems from the deceptive use of the calls cannot be determined until these latter possibilities are rigorously tested. Am. J. Primatol. 75:715-725, 2013. © 2012 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

Highlights

  • Communication involves interactions between at least two individuals, the signaler and the receiver

  • In the case of tufted capuchins, calls given in response to terrestrial predators are of the same general call type as those given in response to aggression and spontaneously during competitive feeding [Di Bitetti, 2001], but it is possible that there are some subtle acoustic differences between predator-elicited hiccups and deceptive false alarms

  • The results suggest that the decreased response rate to terrestrial predator-elicited alarm calls during competitive feeding contexts among tufted capuchins [Wheeler, 2010a], when the calls are more likely to be functionally deceptive than indicative of a predator detection [Wheeler, 2009], is better explained by receivers taking contextual cues into account to determine an appropriate response than by perceiving structural differences between predator-elicited and deceptive false alarms

Read more

Summary

Introduction

Communication involves interactions between at least two individuals, the signaler and the receiver. The evolution of communication is a coevolutionary arms race between these two players, with signaling behavior evolving to influence receiver behavior in a way that preferentially benefits the signaler, while receivers evolve responses to signals that increase their own fitness [Johnstone & Grafen, 1993; Krebs & Dawkins, 1984; Seyfarth et al, 2010]. C 2012 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. The degree to which a given signal must be reliable in order to elicit responses will vary from signal to signal, with the costs of ignoring a reliable signal versus those associated with responding to an unreliable one being the critical factors that determine the evolutionary stable ratio [Johnstone & Grafen, 1993; Searcy & Nowicki, 2005; Wiley, 1994].

Methods
Results
Conclusion
Full Text
Published version (Free)

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call