Abstract

In lateral interception tasks balls converging onto the same interception location via different trajectories give rise to systematic differences in the kinematics of hand movement. While it is generally accepted that this angle-of-approach effect reflects the prospective (on-line) control of movement, controversy exists with respect to the information used to guide the hand to the future interception location. Based on the pattern of errors observed in a task requiring visual extrapolation of line segments to their intersection with a second line, angle-of-approach effects in lateral interception have been argued to result from perceptual biases in the detection of information about the ball's future passing distance along the axis of hand movement. Here we demonstrate that this account does not hold under experimental scrutiny: The angle-of-approach effect still emerged when participants intercepted balls moving along trajectories characterized by a zero perceptual bias with respect to the ball's future arrival position (Experiment 4). Designing and validating such bias-controlled trajectories were done using the line-intersection extrapolation task (Experiments 2 and 3). The experimental set-up used in the present series of experiments was first validated for the lateral interception and the line-intersection extrapolation tasks: In Experiment 1 we used rectilinear ball trajectories to replicate the angle-of-approach effect in lateral interception of virtual balls. Using line segments extracted from these rectilinear ball trajectories, in Experiment 2 we replicated the reported pattern of errors in the estimated locus of intersection with the axis of hand movement. We used these errors to develop a set of bias-free trajectories. Experiment 3 confirmed that the perceptual biases had been corrected for successfully. We discuss the implications on the information-based regulation of hand movement of our finding that the angle-of-approach effect in lateral interception cannot not explained by perceptual biases in information about the ball's future passing distance.

Highlights

  • Success in interceptive actions requires getting to the right place at the right time [1]

  • While the correlation between Constant Error and Segment Orientation was significant for the 3.0-cm segment distance, the slope of the relation indicated that this effect was negligible for the present purposes: a 45u-variation in Segment Orientation was associated with a change in Constant Error of less than 0.05 cm

  • The angle-of-approach effect observed in Experiment 1 continued to emerge even when participants intercepted ball moving along the bias-controlled trajectories

Read more

Summary

Introduction

Success in interceptive actions requires getting to the right place at the right time [1]. The organization of interception movements may be based on accurate perceptual estimates of the future place and time of contact, a large body of work provides convincing evidence for a more robust alternative, based on a continuous, functional coupling between information and movement. For interceptive movements of sufficiently long duration, actions are characterized by the pursuit of particular states of the agent-environment interaction that guarantee (i.e., are lawfully related to) the future achievement of the goal. In a prospective control scheme the unfolding movement is based on time-evolving information with respect to what the agent must do so as to ensure interception, without requiring precise knowledge of when and where this will occur. While prospective strategies have been documented for locomotor (whole-body displacement) interception tasks, both in humans [12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19] and animals [20,21,22,23], here we concentrate on manual (lateral) interception

Methods
Results
Conclusion
Full Text
Paper version not known

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.