Abstract

Transferring current research findings on the topic of learning and memory to “brain-based” learning in schools is of great interest among teachers. However, numerous international studies demonstrate that both pre-service and in-service teachers do not always succeed. Instead, they transfer numerous misconceptions about neuroscience, known as neuromyths, into pedagogical practice. As a result, researchers call for more neuroscience in teacher education in order to create a professional understanding of learning and memory. German pre-service science teachers specializing in biology complete neuroscientific modules (human biology/animal physiology) during their studies because they are expected to teach these topics to their students. Thus, they are required to demonstrate a certain degree of neuroscience literacy. In the present study, 550 pre-service science teachers were surveyed on neuromyths and scientific concepts about learning and memory. Pre-service science teachers’ scientific concepts increased over the course of their training. However, beliefs in neuromyths were independent of participants’ status within teacher education (first-year students, advanced students, and post-graduate trainees). The results showed that 10 neuromyths were endorsed by more than 50% of prospective science teachers. Beliefs in the existence of learning styles (93%) and the effectiveness of Brain Gym (92%) were most widespread. Many myths were endorsed even though a large share of respondents had thematically similar scientific concepts; endorsement of neuromyths was found to be largely independent of professional knowledge as well as theory-based and biography-based learning beliefs about neuroscience and learning. Our results suggest that neuromyths can exist in parallel to scientific concepts, professional knowledge and beliefs and are resistant to formal education. From the perspective of conceptual change theory, they thus exhibit characteristic traits of misconceptions that cannot simply be counteracted with increased neuroscientific knowledge. On the basis of our study’s findings, it can be concluded that new teacher programs considering neuromyths as change-resistant misconceptions are needed to professionalize pre-service science teachers’ neuroscience literacy. For this, an intensive web of exchange between the education field and neuroscientists is required, not just to deploy the latest scientific insights to refute neuromyths on learning and memory, but also to identify further neuromyths.

Highlights

  • Findings from brain research have unleashed a veritable “neuroboom” in recent years, which has taken the form of numerous publications for teachers as well as learning guides for students (e.g., Doyle and Zakrajsek, 2013)

  • Our results indicate that neuromyths can exist in parallel to scientific concepts, professional knowledge and beliefs about neuroscience and learning and are resistant to conventional German teacher education, which promotes many aspects of professional competency on this topic

  • Joint publications and training programs for pre-service and in-service teachers on the cognitive errors involved in neuromyths would be an important first step toward eliminating ‘language barriers’ (Pickering and Howard-Jones, 2007) and closing the gap between neuroscience and the practice of education (Edelenbosch et al., 2015), at least with respect to neuromyths

Read more

Summary

Introduction

Findings from brain research have unleashed a veritable “neuroboom” in recent years, which has taken the form of numerous publications for teachers as well as learning guides for students (e.g., Doyle and Zakrajsek, 2013). Teachers in all subjects have expressed great interest in neuroscience research findings and find it useful to incorporate them into their instruction (Dekker et al, 2012). Even incorrectly interpreted research findings have great appeal once images of the brain and/or neuroscientific explanations are added (McCabe and Castel, 2008; Lindell and Kidd, 2013). Even teachers, the alleged experts on learning, endorse misconceptions about neuroscience and base their pedagogical practice on neuromyths (e.g., Dekker et al, 2012)

Objectives
Results
Discussion
Conclusion

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.