Abstract

In freshwater environments, chemosensory cues play an important role in predator-prey interactions. Prey use a variety of chemosensory cues to detect and avoid predators. However, whether predators use the chemical cues released by disturbed or stressed prey has received less attention. Here we tested the hypothesis that the disturbance cue cortisol, in conjunction with visual cues of prey, elevates predatory behavior. We presented predators (perch, Perca fluviatilis) with three chemosensory choice tests and recorded their location, orientation, and aggressive behavior. We compared the responses of predators when provided with (i) visual cues of prey only (two adjacent tanks containing sticklebacks); (ii) visual and natural chemical cues of prey vs. visual cues only; and (iii) visual cues of prey with cortisol vs. visual cues only. Perch spent a significantly higher proportion of time in proximity to prey, and orientated toward prey more, when presented with a cortisol stimulus plus visual cues, relative to presentations of visual and natural chemical cues of prey, or visual cues of prey only. There was a trend that perch directed a higher proportion of predatory behaviors (number of lunges) toward sticklebacks when presented with a cortisol stimulus plus visual cues, relative to the other chemosensory conditions. But they did not show a significant increase in total predatory behavior in response to cortisol. Therefore, it is not clear whether water-borne cortisol, in conjunction with visual cues of prey, affects predatory behavior. Our results provide evidence that cortisol could be a source of public information about prey state and/or disturbance, but further work is required to confirm this.

Highlights

  • The outcomes of predator–prey interactions are largely influenced by the ability of predators and prey to detect and respond to one another (Lima & Dill, 1990; Endler, 1991)

  • We presented each perch with three chemosensory prey choice tests (i) visual cues only of two adjacent tanks containing sticklebacks; (ii) visual and natural chemical cues of sticklebacks on one side vs. visual cues only on the other; and (iii) visual cues of sticklebacks with a cortisol stimulus on one side vs. visual cues only on the other (Fig. 1)

  • Of the 18 perch, nine did not move during an entire test (n = 12 out of 54 tests), and this was marginally, but non-significantly, more likely to occur in trials when cortisol was the chemical cue

Read more

Summary

Introduction

The outcomes of predator–prey interactions are largely influenced by the ability of predators and prey to detect and respond to one another (Lima & Dill, 1990; Endler, 1991). There is a built-in imbalance between predator and prey in regard to the penalty of failure during an encounter: failure for prey is death, whereas for a predator it is only a lost meal (Dawkins & Krebs, 1979). This asymmetry in the selective pressure on predators and prey, known as the ‘life-dinner’ principle (Dawkins & Krebs, 1979), is reflected in the greater research attention afforded to prey responses over predator behavior (Ferrari, Wisenden & Chivers, 2010). A greater understanding of how predators respond to cues released by prey could provide greater insight into the selective forces that shape the evolution of predator–prey interactions

Methods
Results
Conclusion
Full Text
Published version (Free)

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call