Abstract

Japanese and English use temporal cues within vowels, suggesting an audio-processing advantage for temporally-cued contrasts, while Spanish does not. Using a categorial AXB discrimination task, this study investigated how American English-speaking monolinguals and early and late Spanish-English bilinguals perceive three types of temporally-contrasting Japanese pairs: vowel length (kado/kaado), consonant length (iken/ikken), and syllable number (hjaku/hijaku). All groups performed worse than Japanese controls for the vowel length and syllable number contrasts, but only early bilinguals differed from controls for consonant length. This research contributes to a better understanding of how the first-learned language influences speech perception in a second language.

Highlights

  • Research on cross-linguistic and non-native (L2) speech perception has documented poorer discrimination and identification of L2 contrasts that are not functional in the native (L1) phonology and established that the relationship between the L1 and L2 phonological inventories is highly predictive of speech perception performance in the L2 (Best, 1995; Flege, 1995, 2003)

  • Adults have developed highly automatic selective perception routines (SPRs) for detecting reliable acoustic– phonetic cues to differentiate L1 phonemes in variable and prosodic phonetic contexts. Support for this model comes from behavioral studies (e.g., Kewley-Port et al, 2005; training studies of English vowels on Spanish listeners by Kondaurova and Francis, 2010; on Korean listeners by Nishi and Kewley-Port, 2008) showing that L1 speech perception is highly robust under conditions of high cognitive load and nonoptimal listening conditions

  • The present study extended the findings of Hisagi and Strange (2011) using an online experiment to examine the perception of three types of temporally cued contrasts in Japanese by three groups that differed in their language background history

Read more

Summary

Introduction

Research on cross-linguistic and non-native (L2) speech perception has documented poorer discrimination and identification of L2 contrasts that are not functional in the native (L1) phonology and established that the relationship between the L1 and L2 phonological inventories is highly predictive of speech perception performance in the L2 (Best, 1995; Flege, 1995, 2003). The automatic selective perception (ASP) model (Strange and Shafer, 2008, 2011) was proposed to account for these differences in performance as a function of task, stimuli, and language experience In this model, adults have developed highly automatic selective perception routines (SPRs) for detecting reliable acoustic– phonetic cues to differentiate L1 phonemes in variable and prosodic phonetic contexts. Adults have developed highly automatic selective perception routines (SPRs) for detecting reliable acoustic– phonetic cues to differentiate L1 phonemes in variable and prosodic phonetic contexts Support for this model comes from behavioral studies (e.g., Kewley-Port et al, 2005; training studies of English vowels on Spanish listeners by Kondaurova and Francis, 2010; on Korean listeners by Nishi and Kewley-Port, 2008) showing that L1 speech perception is highly robust under conditions of high cognitive load (increased task difficulty and stimulus complexity) and nonoptimal listening conditions (e.g., background noise). L2 learners show marked decrements in speech perception performance as cognitive load is increased. Strange (2011) hypothesized that non-native listeners need to use focal attention to identify the relevant L2 phonetic cues, even when the stimuli and task are relatively simple

Methods
Results
Conclusion
Full Text
Paper version not known

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.