Abstract

Paul et al. published in the August 2002 issue of the Journal a potentially misleading assertion, specifically, that literature concerning sexual orientation and suicide risks has been “primarily based on opportunistic samples.”1 The assertion is neither new nor correct. Similar criticisms were made in 1995 by attendees at a workshop convened by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, the National Institute of Mental Health, and the American Association of Suicidology. These critics argued that the absence of population-based studies made unreliable the emerging findings of elevated risks of suicidality among sexual minority youths. Several years ago, Remafedi noted that the critics’ assertion was premature2: at least 6 peer-reviewed, population-based, or twincontrol studies had been published by 1999. All had corroborated the findings of earlier studies. The quantity and quality of population-based, peer-reviewed research specific to sexual orientation and suicidality published since 19983 make problematic any assertion that could lead readers or researchers to doubt the aggregate reliability of findings on this topic.4 On the basis of the reliability of the relationship between sexual orientation and suicide risks found in the peer-reviewed literature,5 a number of us have begun to study the extent to which the findings can be generalized to sexual minorities in other parts of the world6 as well as ethnically diverse populations in the United States.7 Our initial attempts may well have to rely on “convenience” or “opportunistic” samples—until decision makers include in more population-based surveys questions specific to both sexual orientation and mental health. Paul et al. addressed another important aspect of generalizability, specifically, age cohort effects. One would hope that future researchers will demonstrate a somewhat greater degree of sensitivity to the influence of cultural contexts for psychological issues in gay and lesbian studies.8 It makes little sense to impose 10-year intervals on data to examine cohort effects among a minority group that has experienced substantial oppression, discrimination, and stigma. It would be more sensible to examine cohorts that reached varying developmental life stages (e.g., adolescence or onset of middle adulthood at age 40) during culturally significant healthrelated events (e.g., the removal of homosexuality from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders [DSM] in 1973 or the removal of egodystonic homosexuality from the DSM in 1987). This tactic is essential for evaluating the extent to which variations in oppression moderate the relationship between sexual orientation and stress-related disorders.9

Full Text
Published version (Free)

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call