Abstract
Two pieces of information are absolutely required to locate a source of chemicals from a distance. The first and most obvious is the presence of the chemical cue, and the second is the direction of the flow of air or water carrying the chemicals. Insects orienting to wind while walking in terrestrial environments appear to use the mechanical deflection of their antennae to detect the direction and magnitude of the flow they are moving through (Bell and Kramer, 1979). Thus, in these cases the antennae are providing information on both the presence (and absence) of odor and the direction of the flow. Many flying insects locate resources such as food and mates using windborne odors (Willis and Arbas, 1991; Arbas et al., 1993). Flying insects also use mechanosensory deflection of their antennae, in addition to deflection of mechanosensory hairs on their faces, to detect their speed and direction of movement through the air (Arbas, 1986). This information is then integrated with visually detected flow field feedback showing their movement with respect to the environment (David, 1986) to determine their steering movements (Olberg, 1983). Thus, the visual flow field feedback used to stabilize and control their flight behaviour also provides the information on wind speed and direction necessary to steer and control flight with respect to the wind during odor tracking behaviour (i.e. wind information is the result of the difference between intended and actual course) (David, 1986). One of the most studied flying odor tracking systems is that of male moths tracking female sex attractant pheromone to locate mates (Arbas et al., 1993). Perhaps the most remarkable olfactory task achieved by insects is that of locating distant resources using airborne chemical cues. It is currently thought that two primary mechanisms underlie flying insects’ ability to perform this behavior: (i) an odor-modulated, visually mediated orientation to the wind direction; and (ii) an odoractivated internal program of turns. The blending of these two mechanisms is thought to generate the upwind zigzagging paths that we observe from insects tracking odors in flight (Figure 1B). It is well established that these two mechanisms are both modulated by characteristics of odor plumes, resulting in predictable changes in the structure of the tracking behavior. It is not yet clear whether vision contributes significantly to the odor tracking behavior of walking insects. However, recent experiments revealed that the structure of the walking paths of pheromone tracking cockroaches look remarkably like those observed from flying insects, and thus may be governed by similar mechanisms (Figure 1A). These new results suggest that zigzagging into the wind may possess elements that enable odor trackers to more successfully adapt their behavior and locate important resources using odor information. We are using robots that can move in two and three dimensions to test hypothetical control algorithms based on our results and the results of similar behavioral studies. Our recent experiments with wheeled robots tracking wind-borne plumes of ionized air (Figure 1C) have shown that there are tradeoffs between algorithms that generate rapid tracking but low probabilities of source location, and algorithms that generate very slow tracking but much higher probabilities of source location. While algorithms that alter the robot’s steering and turning behavior according to the local plume concentration generate tracks that are both rapid and highly successful at locating the source. Wheeled robots are a good way to test hypothetical control algorithms underlying the olfactory orientation of walking insects, but not for flying insects. We have recently developed a gantry robot that is capable of tracking windborne plumes in three dimensions (Figure 1D). This robot will eventually have all of the sensors possessed by the pheromone tracking moths and use them to control its motion.
Talk to us
Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have
Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.