Abstract

Objective: This study investigated whether individuals reporting vicarious pain in daily life (e.g., the self-reported vicarious pain group) display vicarious experiences during an experimental paradigm, and also show an improved detection of somatosensory stimuli while observing another in pain. Furthermore, this study investigated the stability of these phenomena. Finally, this study explored the putative modulating role of dispositional empathy and hypervigilance for pain.Methods: Vicarious pain responders (i.e., reporting vicarious pain in daily life; N = 16) and controls (N = 19) were selected from a large sample, and viewed videos depicting pain-related (hands being pricked) and non-pain related scenes, whilst occasionally experiencing vibrotactile stimuli themselves on the left, right or both hands. Participants reported the location at which they felt a somatosensory stimulus. We calculated the number of vicarious errors (i.e., the number of trials in which an illusionary sensation was reported while observing pain-related scenes) and detection accuracy. Thirty-three participants (94.29%) took part in the same experiment 5 months later to investigate the temporal stability of the outcomes.Results: The vicarious pain group reported more vicarious errors compared with controls and this effect proved to be stable over time. Detection was facilitated while observing pain-related scenes compared with non-pain related scenes. Observers' characteristics, i.e., dispositional empathy and hypervigilance for pain, did not modulate the effects.Conclusion: Observing pain facilitates the detection of tactile stimuli, both in vicarious pain responders and controls. Interestingly, vicarious pain responders reported more vicarious errors during the experimental paradigm compared to controls and this effect remained stable over time.

Highlights

  • Evidence reveals that similar brain areas are activated when observing pain in another and when experiencing pain ourselves (Jackson et al, 2006; Bufalari et al, 2007; Gu and Han, 2007; Keysers et al, 2010; Lamm and Singer, 2010; Corradi-Dell’Acqua et al, 2011; Lamm et al, 2011)

  • The vicarious pain group reported more vicarious errors compared with controls and this effect proved to be stable over time

  • Vicarious pain responders reported more vicarious errors during the experimental paradigm compared to controls and this effect remained stable over time

Read more

Summary

Introduction

Evidence reveals that similar brain areas are activated when observing pain in another and when experiencing pain ourselves (Jackson et al, 2006; Bufalari et al, 2007; Gu and Han, 2007; Keysers et al, 2010; Lamm and Singer, 2010; Corradi-Dell’Acqua et al, 2011; Lamm et al, 2011). Individuals are asked to rate whether they experience vicarious sensations in specific situations or in daily life Based upon these ratings, participants are selected in a second phase, to take part for example in neuroimaging (e.g., Osborn and Derbyshire, 2010) or in an experimental study (e.g., Vandenbroucke et al, 2013). To our knowledge, no study has examined whether the report of vicarious experiences is stable over time

Methods
Results
Conclusion
Full Text
Paper version not known

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.