Abstract

Back to table of contents Previous article Next article LettersFull AccessNumbers Written Counterclockwise in the Clock Drawing TestHui-Ju Kao, M.D., and Tung-Hua Chiang, M.D.Hui-Ju KaoSearch for more papers by this author, M.D., and Tung-Hua ChiangSearch for more papers by this author, M.D.Published Online:1 Jan 2014https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.neuropsych.13030052AboutSectionsView articleSupplemental MaterialPDF/EPUB ToolsAdd to favoritesDownload CitationsTrack Citations ShareShare onFacebookTwitterLinked InEmail View articleTo the Editor: A recent review (Windows to the Brain: The Clock Drawing Task: Common Errors and Functional Neuroanatomy)1 described Rouleau’s qualitative system and the relationship between clock-drawing errors and brain networks. Frontoparietal and frontostriatal circuits are important for visuospatial and executive functions necessary for accurate clock-drawing. We highlight the error of counterclockwise numbers as spatial and/or planning deficits according to Rouleau’s system and argue that most of correctly sequenced numbers 1–12 written counterclockwise may suggest memory-system involvement.There are various number-order errors in the clock drawing test (CDT; Figure 1), and examples of representative clock-drawing appear in the supplement attached to this letter. Mendez suggested that >6 correctly sequenced of the same symbol type falls under the concept of “numerical factors,” which encode semantic and spatially-coded knowledge that a clock-face contains the numbers 1–12 arranged to represent hours.2FIGURE 1. Relationships Among Number-Order Errors in the Clock Drawing TestThe reversed clock phenomenon (RCP) is an extreme example, in which 1–12 are written counterclockwise. Stroke patients with RCP often have lesions involving the right parietal lobe and subcortical area. Few stroke patients have parietotemporal lobe lesions, but lesions that involve multiple cortices make it difficult to clarify the role of the temporal lobes. RCP stroke patients show evidence of unilateral spatial neglect. Attentional orientation and visuospatial dysfunction, especially spatial representation, have been implicated in RCP stroke cases. Nevertheless, patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and RCP did not show unilateral spatial neglect in Brugnolo’s study.3 The involvement of temporal lobes in AD patients with RCP supports the view that they are important for the visual-semantic retrieval of clock representation in AD patients.The CDT can involve an instruction to draw a clock-face from memory (i.e. draw-to-command). Alternatively, a subject can be asked to copy a clock (i.e., copy-to-command), which places less demand on executive functions and semantic memory.1 RCP or counterclockwise writing by dementia patients was mostly in the clockwise order for the copy-to-command. Two subacute stroke patients and RCP with different lesions can produce similar clocks for the draw-to-command (i.e., allochiria), but different clocks for the draw-to-copy.4,5 We suggest that the combination of copy-to-command and draw-to-command provides more information about the availability of cognitive functions.The CDT is simple and useful for monitoring dynamic cognitive change. A further understanding of the CDT results will help us design clinical rehabilitation programs to address specific cognitive impairments.Dept. of Neurology, Taichung Wizcare Hospital, Taichung City 404, TaiwanDept. of Neurology, Cheng-Ching Hospital, Taichung City 400, TaiwanCorrespondence: Hui-Ju Kao, M.D.; e-mail: kaohj.[email protected]comReferences1 Eknoyan D, Hurley RA, Taber KH: The clock drawing task: common errors and functional neuroanatomy. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2012; 24:260–265Link, Google Scholar2 Mendez MF, Ala T, Underwood KL: Development of scoring criteria for the clock drawing task in Alzheimer’s disease. J Am Geriatr Soc 1992; 40:1095–1099Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar3 Brugnolo A, Morbelli S, Dessi B, et al.: The reversed clock drawing test phenomenon in Alzheimer’s disease: a perfusion SPECT study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2010; 29:1–10Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar4 Lepore M, Conson M, Grossi D, et al.: On the different mechanisms of spatial transpositions: a case of representational allochiria in clock drawing. Neuropsychologia 2003; 41:1290–1295Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar5 Lepore M, Conson M, Ferrigno A, et al.: Spatial transpositions across tasks and response modalities: exploring representational allochiria. Neurocase 2004; 10:386–392Crossref, Medline, Google Scholar FiguresReferencesCited byDetailsCited ByNone Volume 26Issue 1 Winter 2014Pages E48-E49 Metrics PDF download History Published online 1 January 2014 Published in print 1 January 2014

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call

Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.