Abstract
In the last decade, educational neuroscience has become increasingly important in the context of instruction, and its applications have been transformed into new teaching methods. Although teachers are interested in educational neuroscience, communication between scientists and teachers is not always straightforward. Thus, misunderstandings of neuroscientific research results can evolve into so-called neuromyths. The aim of the present study was to investigate the prevalence of such music-related neuromyths among music teachers and music students. Based on an extensive literature research, 26 theses were compiled and subsequently evaluated by four experts. Fourteen theses were selected, of which seven were designated as scientifically substantiated and seven as scientifically unsubstantiated (hereafter labeled as “neuromyths”). One group of adult music teachers (n = 91) and one group of music education students (n = 125) evaluated the theses (forced-choice discrimination task) in two separate online surveys. Additionally, in both surveys person-characteristic variables were gathered to determine possible predictors for the discrimination performance. As a result, identification rates of the seven scientifically substantiated theses were similar for teachers (76%) and students (78%). Teachers and students correctly rejected 60 and 59%, respectively, of the seven neuromyths as scientifically unsubstantiated statements. Sensitivity analysis by signal detection theory revealed a discrimination performance of d' = 1.25 (SD = 1.12) for the group of teachers and d' = 1.48 (SD = 1.22) for the students. Both groups showed a general tendency to evaluate the theses as scientifically substantiated (teachers: c = −0.35, students: c = −0.41). Specifically, buzz words such as “brain hemisphere” or “cognitive enhancement” were often classified as correct. For the group of teachers, the best predictor of discrimination performance was having read a large number of media about educational neuroscience and related topics (R2 = 0.06). For the group of students, the best predictors for discrimination performance were a high number of read media and the hitherto completed number of semesters (R2 = 0.14). Our findings make clear that both teachers and students are far from being experts on topics related to educational neuroscience in music and would therefore benefit from current education-related research in psychology and neuroscience.
Highlights
Educational neuroscience, a comparatively new field, deals with the application of neuroscientific knowledge in educational practice (Alexander et al, 2012)
Since our study was presented as a study about neuro-educational knowledge, no suspicion was raised among the participants
The percentage of correct answers relating to neuromyths and relating to scientifically substantiated theses reveals that teachers are more uncertain in evaluating neuromyths than in evaluating scientifically substantiated theses, as was hypothesized
Summary
Educational neuroscience, a comparatively new field, deals with the application of neuroscientific knowledge in educational practice (Alexander et al, 2012). Neuroscience has come to wide attention since the 1990s, due to new research made possible by a higher availability of neuro-imaging methods, such as fMRI. Misconceptions can occur at any of the mentioned stages of communication Such misconceptions have been labeled as neuromyths (Goswami, 2006) and are generated by the misinterpreting, simplifying, misunderstanding and, in some cases, deliberate warping of neuroscientific research results People notice and memorize information that supports their own assumptions more often than contradicting information They often mistake causation for correlation (Lilienfeld et al, 2010, pp. 9–19)
Talk to us
Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have
Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.