Abstract
Understanding the neuroanatomical correlates of individual differences in executive function (EF) is integral to a complete characterization of the neural systems supporting cognition. While studies have investigated EF-neuroanatomy relationships in adults, these studies often include samples with wide variation in age, which may mask relationships between neuroanatomy and EF specific to certain neurodevelopmental time points, and such studies often use unreliable single task measures of EF. Here we address both issues. First, we focused on a specific age at which the majority of neurodevelopmental changes are complete but at which age-related atrophy is not likely (N = 251; mean age of 28.71 years, SD = 0.57). Second, we assessed EF through multiple tasks, deriving three factors scores guided by the unity/diversity model of EF, which posits a common EF factor that influences all EF tasks, as well as an updating-specific and shifting-specific factor. We found that better common EF was associated with greater volume and surface area of regions in right middle frontal gyrus/frontal pole, right inferior temporal gyrus, as well as fractional anisotropy in portions of the right superior longitudinal fasciculus (rSLF) and the left anterior thalamic radiation. Better updating-specific ability was associated with greater cortical thickness of a cluster in left cuneus/precuneus, and reduced cortical thickness in regions of right superior frontal gyrus and right middle/superior temporal gyrus, but no aspects of white matter diffusion. In contrast, better shifting-specific ability was not associated with gray matter characteristics, but rather was associated with increased mean diffusivity and reduced radial diffusivity throughout much of the brain and reduced axial diffusivity in distinct clusters of the left superior longitudinal fasciculus, the corpus callosum, and the right optic radiation. These results demonstrate that associations between individual differences in EF ability and regional neuroanatomical properties occur not only within classic brain networks thought to support EF, but also in a variety of other regions and white matter tracts. These relationships appear to differ from observations made in emerging adults (Smolker et al., 2015), which might indicate that the brain systems associated with EF continue to experience behaviorally relevant maturational process beyond the early 20s.
Highlights
Executive function (EF) is a set of domain-general cognitive control mechanisms supporting goal-directed behaviors (Banich, 2009)
While the relationship with the entire right superior longitudinal fasciculus (rSLF) was only marginal in the current sample, when we ran voxel-wise FA analyses within a mask of the rSLF, we found a significant positive association between a portion of SLF-II and common executive function (EF), suggesting that the rSLF is important to individual differences in common EF across both emerging and young adulthood
Within a sample of developmentally mature young adults, common EF and updating-specific were associated with distinct properties of regional gray matter morphometry and the location of these features fell both within and outside of the fronto-parietal network (FPN)
Summary
Executive function (EF) is a set of domain-general cognitive control mechanisms supporting goal-directed behaviors (Banich, 2009). Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies evaluating the neural substrates associated with distinct EF dimensions have implicated regions of lateral PFC (Duncan and Owen, 2000; Wager and Smith, 2003; Collette et al, 2005; Petrides, 2005; Banich, 2009) and medial PFC (Wager et al, 2004; Derrfuss et al, 2005) in supporting discrete EF constructs analogous to those evaluated in the current study. Such findings would suggest that higher levels of EF are characterized by the potential for larger participation and/or distribution of processing across the brain
Talk to us
Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have
Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.