Abstract
Interactions between the body and the environment occur within the peripersonal space (PPS), the space immediately surrounding the body. The PPS is encoded by multisensory (audio-tactile, visual-tactile) neurons that possess receptive fields (RFs) anchored on the body and restricted in depth. The extension in depth of PPS neurons’ RFs has been documented to change dynamically as a function of the velocity of incoming stimuli, but the underlying neural mechanisms are still unknown. Here, by integrating a psychophysical approach with neural network modeling, we propose a mechanistic explanation behind this inherent dynamic property of PPS. We psychophysically mapped the size of participant’s peri-face and peri-trunk space as a function of the velocity of task-irrelevant approaching auditory stimuli. Findings indicated that the peri-trunk space was larger than the peri-face space, and, importantly, as for the neurophysiological delineation of RFs, both of these representations enlarged as the velocity of incoming sound increased. We propose a neural network model to mechanistically interpret these findings: the network includes reciprocal connections between unisensory areas and higher order multisensory neurons, and it implements neural adaptation to persistent stimulation as a mechanism sensitive to stimulus velocity. The network was capable of replicating the behavioral observations of PPS size remapping and relates behavioral proxies of PPS size to neurophysiological measures of multisensory neurons’ RF size. We propose that a biologically plausible neural adaptation mechanism embedded within the network encoding for PPS can be responsible for the dynamic alterations in PPS size as a function of the velocity of incoming stimuli.NEW & NOTEWORTHY Interactions between body and environment occur within the peripersonal space (PPS). PPS neurons are highly dynamic, adapting online as a function of body-object interactions. The mechanistic underpinning PPS dynamic properties are unexplained. We demonstrate with a psychophysical approach that PPS enlarges as incoming stimulus velocity increases, efficiently preventing contacts with faster approaching objects. We present a neurocomputational model of multisensory PPS implementing neural adaptation to persistent stimulation to propose a neurophysiological mechanism underlying this effect.
Highlights
All interactions between an agent and the environment are mediated by the body and occur within the peripersonal space (PPS), that is, the space immediately adjacent to and surrounding the body (di Pellegrino et al 1997; Rizzolatti et al 1981, 1997)
Overall participants demonstrated to be very accurate at withholding response during unimodal auditory catch trials, and results were solely analyzed in terms of Reaction times (RTs)
With regard to the RTs to touch on the face as a function of the distance of task-irrelevant sounds, a repeated-measures ANOVA demonstrated a main effect of distance (F6,90 ϭ 41.56, P Ͻ 0.001, partial 2 ϭ 0.73), and a distance ϫ sound velocity interaction (F6,90 ϭ 3.79, P ϭ 0.002, partial 2 ϭ 0.20), yet no main effect of sound velocity (F1,15 Ͻ 1, P ϭ 0.99)
Summary
All interactions between an agent and the environment are mediated by the body and occur within the peripersonal space (PPS), that is, the space immediately adjacent to and surrounding the body (di Pellegrino et al 1997; Rizzolatti et al 1981, 1997). The ecological relevance of the near space is evidenced in that the primate brain has developed a frontoparietal network dedicated to the representation of PPS (Graziano et al 1997; Graziano and Cooke 2006; Grivaz et al 2017) Neurons within this network are multisensory (Avillac et al 2007; Bernasconi et al 2018), in that they respond to tactile stimulation on the body, and to visual (Duhamel et al 1997, 1998; Schlack et al 2005) or auditory stimuli (Graziano et al 1999) presented close to, but not far from, the body.
Talk to us
Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have
Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.