Abstract

Feedback is beneficial for learning. Nevertheless, it remains unclear whether (i) feedback draws attentional resources when integrated and (ii) the benefits of feedback for learning can be demonstrated using an attention-based task. We therefore (i) isolated feedback-specific load from task-specific load via individual differences in attention resource capacity and (ii) examined the effect of trial-by-trial feedback (i.e., present vs. absent) on learning a multiple object-tracking (MOT) paradigm. We chose MOT because it is a robust measure of attention resource capacity. In Study 1 participants tracked one (i.e., lowest attentional load condition) through four target items (i.e., highest load condition) among eight total items. One group (n = 32) received trial-by-trial feedback whereas the other group (n = 32) did not. The absence of feedback resulted in better MOT performance compared with the presence of feedback. Moreover, the difference in MOT capability between groups increased as the task-specific attentional load increased. These findings suggest that feedback integration requires attentional resources. Study 2 examined whether the absence (n = 19) or presence (n = 19) of feedback affects learning on the same MOT task across four testing days. When holding task-specific load constant, improvement in MOT was greater with feedback than without. Although this study is the first to isolate feedback-specific load in attention with MOT, more evidence is needed to demonstrate how the benefits of feedback translate to improvement on an attention-based task. These findings encourage future research to further explore the interaction between feedback, attention and learning.

Highlights

  • Learning can be accelerated by providing additional information immediately after a task is completed (Wilbert, Grosche, & Gerdes, 2010)

  • The current study examined the role of feedback on attention and learning using a multiple object-tracking (MOT) task

  • Using a MOT paradigm, Study 1 demonstrated that feedback requires its share of attentional resources for the participant to process this additive information

Read more

Summary

Introduction

Learning can be accelerated by providing additional information immediately after a task is completed (Wilbert, Grosche, & Gerdes, 2010). Successfully completing a task requires one to allocate sufficient cognitive resources to match the task’s cognitive demands, and learning is reliant on one’s domain-specific resource capacity. A task’s level of difficulty, cognitive demands, or the amount of cognitive resources required to successfully complete the task is referred to as the task’s cognitive load (Paas & van Merriënboer, 1994; Sweller, 1988; Sweller, 1994). The successful completion of a task depends on whether one is able to match the task’s cognitive load from their reserve of cognitive resources (Paas & Van Merriënboer, 1994). If feedback requires its own share of cognitive resources, successfully completing a task would rely on one’s capacity to allocate resources to the feedback-specific load in addition to the task-specific load

Methods
Results
Conclusion
Full Text
Published version (Free)

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call