Abstract

Affective states are known to influence behavior and cognitive processes. To assess mood (moderately long-term affective states), the cognitive judgment bias test was developed and has been widely used in various animal species. However, little is known about how mood changes, how mood can be experimentally manipulated, and how mood then feeds back into cognitive judgment. A recent theory argues that mood reflects the cumulative impact of differences between obtained outcomes and expectations. Here expectations refer to an established context. Situations in which an established context fails to match an outcome are then perceived as mismatches of expectation and outcome. We take advantage of the large number of studies published on non-verbal cognitive bias tests in recent years (95 studies with a total of 162 independent tests) to test whether cumulative mismatch could indeed have led to the observed mood changes. Based on a criteria list, we assessed whether mismatch had occurred with the experimental procedure used to induce mood (mood induction mismatch), or in the context of the non-verbal cognitive bias procedure (testing mismatch). For the mood induction mismatch, we scored the mismatch between the subjects’ potential expectations and the manipulations conducted for inducing mood whereas, for the testing mismatch, we scored mismatches that may have occurred during the actual testing. We then investigated whether these two types of mismatch can predict the actual outcome of the cognitive bias study. The present evaluation shows that mood induction mismatch cannot well predict the success of a cognitive bias test. On the other hand, testing mismatch can modulate or even inverse the expected outcome. We think, cognitive bias studies should more specifically aim at creating expectation mismatch while inducing mood states to test the cumulative mismatch theory more properly. Furthermore, testing mismatch should be avoided as much as possible because it can reverse the affective state of animals as measured in a cognitive judgment bias paradigm.

Highlights

  • In the last years, interest in affective states in animals has increased (Boissy et al, 2007; Veissier and Boissy, 2007) because these states are thought to reflect welfare from the perspective of the animals (Kendrick, 2007; Yeates and Main, 2008)

  • With a negative cumulative mood induction mismatch, the success of cognitive bias studies decreased from no testing mismatch and a negative testing mismatch to a positive testing mismatch (χ22 = 19.04, p = 0.00007; Figure 1, right)

  • This implies that, as far as we know, the cognitive bias test outcome results were not influenced by a testing mismatch increasing the probability to see an effect of mood induction mismatch

Read more

Summary

Introduction

Interest in affective states in animals has increased (Boissy et al, 2007; Veissier and Boissy, 2007) because these states are thought to reflect welfare from the perspective of the animals (Kendrick, 2007; Yeates and Main, 2008). It is assumed that different types of affective states are relevant for different periods of time (Spruijt et al, 2001; Posner et al, 2005; Mendl et al, 2009). Affective traits on the other hand are constant over long periods of time and specific to each individual (Luthans and Youssef, 2007; Kluember et al, 2009; Faustino et al, 2015). We refer to mood as an affective state that persists over some time rather than being a fleeting response to a single event (Luthans and Youssef, 2007; Kivumbi, 2011; Faustino et al, 2015)

Objectives
Methods
Results
Discussion
Conclusion
Full Text
Published version (Free)

Talk to us

Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have

Schedule a call