Abstract
The distributed nature of information processing in the brain creates a complex variety of decision making behavior. Likewise, computational models of saccadic decision making behavior are numerous and diverse. Here we present a generative model of saccadic action selection in the context of competitive decision making in the superior colliculus (SC) in order to investigate how independent neural signals may converge to interact and guide saccade selection, and to test if systematic variations can better replicate the variability in responses that are part of normal human behavior. The model was tasked with performing pro- and anti-saccades in order to replicate specific attributes of healthy human saccade behavior. Participants (ages 18–39) were instructed to either look toward (pro-saccade, well-practiced automated response) or away from (anti-saccade, combination of inhibitory and voluntary responses) a peripheral visual stimulus. They generated express and regular latency saccades in the pro-saccade task. In the anti-saccade task, correct reaction times were longer and participants occasionally looked at the stimulus (direction error) at either express or regular latencies. To gain a better understanding of the underlying neural processes that lead to saccadic action selection and response inhibition, we implemented 8 inputs inspired by systems neuroscience. These inputs reflected known sensory, automated, voluntary, and inhibitory components of cortical and basal ganglia activity that coalesces in the intermediate layers of the SC (SCi). The model produced bimodal reaction time distributions, where express and regular latency saccades had distinct modes, for both correct pro-saccades and direction errors in the anti-saccade task. Importantly, express and regular latency direction errors resulted from interactions of different inputs in the model. Express latency direction errors were due to a lack of pre-emptive fixation and inhibitory activity, which aloud sensory and automated inputs to initiate a stimulus-driven saccade. Regular latency errors occurred when the automated motor signals were stronger than the voluntary motor signals. While previous models have emulated fewer aspects of these behavioral findings, the focus of the simulations here is on the interaction of a wide variety of physiologically-based information integration producing a richer set of natural behavioral variability.
Highlights
Saccadic action-selection requires a complex network of brain areas that provide different information signals
We argue that there are different types of response inhibition used to prevent express vs. regular latency direction errors
We have formalized five specific types of saccadic behaviors observed in the pro- and anti-saccade tasks based on saccadic reaction times (SRT) and the saccade selected: 1) Regular latency correct pro-saccades (Regular-Pro) 2) Express latency correct pro-saccades (Express-Pro) 3) All correct anti-saccades (Correct-Anti) 4) Regular latency anti-saccade direction errors (Regular-Errors) 5) Express latency anti-saccade direction errors (Express-Errors)
Summary
Saccadic action-selection requires a complex network of brain areas that provide different information signals (e.g., sensory, motor, cognitive). Detailed analysis of the temporal distributions of correct pro- and antisaccades and direction errors in healthy humans and patients with neurological disease can be used to identify different signals coding response inhibition and saccade generation that contribute to the natural behavioral repertoire produced in these tasks (Coe and Munoz, 2017). In this paper we introduce a computational model of action selection, based on Trappenberg et al (2001), that includes conceptualized inputs inspired by sensory, automated, voluntary, and inhibitory components of neural activity. This model is intended to isolate the various types of information coalescing within the primate brain to guide saccadic action selection.
Talk to us
Join us for a 30 min session where you can share your feedback and ask us any queries you have
Disclaimer: All third-party content on this website/platform is and will remain the property of their respective owners and is provided on "as is" basis without any warranties, express or implied. Use of third-party content does not indicate any affiliation, sponsorship with or endorsement by them. Any references to third-party content is to identify the corresponding services and shall be considered fair use under The CopyrightLaw.